cross-cultural; cumulative culture; innovation; overimitation; sex differences; tool-use; Multidisciplinary
Abstract :
[en] Children are skilful at acquiring tool-using skills by faithfully copying relevant and irrelevant actions performed by others, but poor at innovating tools to solve problems. Five- to twelve-year-old urban French and rural Serbian children (N = 208) were exposed to a Hook task; a jar containing a reward in a bucket and a pipe cleaner as potential recovering tool material. In both countries, few children under the age of 10 made a hook from the pipe cleaner to retrieve the reward on their own. However, from five onward, the majority of unsuccessful children succeeded after seeing an adult model manufacturing a hook without completing the task. Additionally, a third of the children who observed a similar demonstration including an irrelevant action performed with a second object, a string, replicated this meaningless action. Children's difficulty with innovation and early capacity for overimitation thus do not depend on socio-economic background. Strikingly, we document a sex difference in overimitation across cultures, with boys engaging more in overimitation than girls, a finding that may result from differences regarding explorative tool-related behaviour. This male-biased sex effect sheds new light on our understanding of overimitation, and more generally, on how human tool culture evolved.
Disciplines :
Social, industrial & organizational psychology
Author, co-author :
Frick, Aurélien ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département des sciences cliniques ; Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK ; Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, Suor Orsola Benincasa University, Naples, Italy
Clément, Fabrice; Cognitive Science Centre, University of Neuchâtel, Neuchâtel, Switzerland
Gruber, Thibaud; Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland ; Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Language :
English
Title :
Evidence for a sex effect during overimitation: boys copy irrelevant modelled actions more than girls across cultures.
People Programme SNF - Schweizerischer Nationalfonds zur Förderung der wissenschaftlichen Forschung
Funding text :
The research leading to these results has received funding from the People Programme (Marie Curie Actions) and from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme for research, technological development, and demonstration under REA grant agreement no. 329197 awarded to T.G. During the writing of this article, A.F. was supported by a doctoral scholarship from Suor Orsola Benincasa University, and T.G. was supported by an Interdisciplinary Project CR13I1_162720/1 grant and an Advanced Postdoc. Mobility fellowship P300PA_164678/1 from the Swiss National Science Foundation.Ethics. A written parental consent was obtained for each child and the research project and protocols were reviewed and accepted by the Ethics Committee at the University of Neuchâtel as well as the participating schools. Data accessibility. The data that support the findings of this study are available through the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/2e86d [57]. Authors’ contributions. A.F. and F.C. conceived and designed the study. A.F. collected and coded the data. A.F. and T.G. analysed the data. A.F., T.G. and F.C. wrote the paper. All authors reviewed and approved the final manuscript. Competing interests. We have no competing interests. Funding. The research leading to these results has received funding from the People Programme (Marie Curie Actions) and from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme for research, technological development, and demonstration under REA grant agreement no. 329197 awarded to T.G. During the writing of this article, A.F. was supported by a doctoral scholarship from Suor Orsola Benincasa University, and T.G. was supported by an Interdisciplinary Project CR13I1_162720/1 grant and an Advanced Postdoc. Mobility fellowship P300PA_164678/1 from the Swiss National Science Foundation. Acknowledgements. A.F. wishes to thank particularly Linda Livingstone and Katarina Živkovic for data collection in Serbia, Emilie Sandrin for the double coding and inter-reliability index, Nicolas Chevalier, Diane Austry and
Henrich J. 2015 The secret of our success: how culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Dean LG, Vale GL, Laland KN, Flynn E, Kendal RL. 2014 Human cumulative culture: a comparative perspective. Biol. Rev. 89, 284–301. (doi:10.1111/brv.12053)
Legare CH, Nielsen M. 2015 Imitation and innovation: the dual engines of cultural learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 688–699. (doi:10.1016/j.tics. 2015.08.005)
Heyes C. 2015 When does social learning become cultural learning? Dev. Sci. 20, e12350 (doi:10.1111/desc.12350)
Gruber T, Deschenaux A, Frick A, Clément F. 2017 Group membership influences more social identification than social learning or overimitation in children. Child Dev. Online early view. (doi:10.1111/cdev.12931)
Horner V, Whiten A. 2005 Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Anim. Cogn. 8, 164–181. (doi:10.1007/s10071-004-0239-6)
Lyons DE, Young AG, Keil FC. 2007 The hidden structure of overimitation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 19 751–19 756. (doi:10.1073/pnas.070445 2104)
Gruber T, Zuberbühler K, Clément F, van Schaik C. 2015 Apes have culture but may not know that they do. Front. Psychol. 6, 91. (doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015. 00091)
Lyons DE, Damrosch DH, Lin JK, Macris DM, Keil FC. 2011 The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 1158–1167. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0335)
Over H, Carpenter M. 2012 Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. J. Comp. Psychol. 126, 182–192. (doi:10.1037/a0024555)
Keupp S, Behne T, Rakoczy H. 2013 Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 116, 392–406. (doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013. 07.002)
Nielsen M, Blank C. 2011 Imitation in young children: when who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Dev. Psychol. 47, 1050–1053. (doi:10.1037/a0023866)
McGuigan N, Whiten A, Flynn E, Horner V. 2007 Imitation of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 3- and 5-year-old children. Cogn. Dev. 22, 353–364. (doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2007. 01.001)
Keupp S, Behne T, Zachow J, Kasbohm A, Rakoczy H. 2015 Over-imitation is not automatic: context sensitivity in children’s overimitation and action interpretation of causally irrelevant actions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 130, 163–175. (doi:10.1016/j.jecp. 2014.10.005)
Zmyj N, Daum MM, Aschersleben G. 2009 The development of rational imitation in 9- and 12-month-old infants. Infancy 14, 131–141. (doi:10.1080/15250000802569884)
Flynn S, Smith K. 2012 Investigating the mechanisms of cultural acquisition: how pervasive is overimitation in adults? Soc. Psychol. 43, 185–195. (doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000119)
Keupp S, Bancken C, Schillmöller J, Rakoczy H, Behne T. 2016 Rational over-imitation: preschoolers consider material costs and copy causally irrelevant actions selectively. Cognition 147, 85–92. (doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.11.007)
Kenward B, Karlsson M, Persson J. 2011 Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 1239–1246. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1399)
Marsh LE, Ropar D, Hamilton AFC. 2014 The social modulation of imitation fidelity in school-age children. PLoS ONE 9, e86127. (doi:10.1371/journal. pone.0086127)
Nielsen M. 2013 Young children’s imitative and innovative behaviour on the floating object task. Infant Child Dev. 22, 44–52. (doi:10.1002/icd.1765)
Hanus D, Mendes N, Tennie C, Call J. 2011 Comparing the performances of apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens) in the floating peanut task. PLoS ONE 6, e19555. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0019555)
Meltzoff AN. 1995 Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Dev. Psychol. 31, 838–850. (doi:10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838)
Taniguchi Y, Sanefuji W. 2017 The boundaries of overimitation in preschool children: effects of target and tool use on imitation of irrelevant actions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 159, 83–95. (doi:10.1016/j.jecp. 2017.01.014)
Shumaker RW, Walkup KR, Beck BB. 2011 Animal tool behavior: the use and manufacture of tools by animals. Baltimore, MD: JHU Press.
Beck SR, Apperly IA, Chappell J, Guthrie C, Cutting N. 2011 Making tools isn’t child’s play. Cognition 119, 301–306. (doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.003)
Weir AA, Chappell J, Kacelnik A. 2002 Shaping of hooks in New Caledonian crows. Science 297, 981. (doi:10.1126/science.1073433)
Chappell J, Cutting N, Apperly IA, Beck SR. 2013 The development of tool manufacture in humans: what helps young children make innovative tools? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 20120409. (doi:10.1098/rstb. 2012.0409)
Cutting N, Apperly IA, Beck SR. 2011 Why do children lack the flexibility to innovate tools? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 109, 497–511. (doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011. 02.012)
Muthukrishna M, Henrich J. 2016 Innovation in the collective brain. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 371, 20150192. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2015.0192)
Berl RE, Hewlett BS. 2015 Cultural variation in the use of overimitation by the Aka and Ngandu of the Congo Basin. PLoS ONE 10, e0120180. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0120180)
Nielsen M, Mushin I, Tomaselli K, Whiten A. 2016 Imitation, collaboration, and their interaction among Western and Indigenous Australian preschool children. Child Dev. 87, 795–806. (doi:10.1111/cdev.12504)
Nielsen M, Mushin I, Tomaselli K, Whiten A. 2014 Where culture takes hold: ‘overimitation’ and its flexible deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen children. Child Dev. 85, 2169–2184. (doi:10.1111/cdev.12265)
Nielsen M, Tomaselli K. 2010 Overimitation in Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychol. Sci. 21, 729–736. (doi:10.1177/0956797610368808)
Fletcher R, Nielsen M. 2012 Product-based television and young children’s pretend play in Australia. J. Child. Media 6, 5–17. (doi:10.1080/17482798.2011. 633397)
Nielsen M, Tomaselli K, Mushin I, Whiten A. 2014 Exploring tool innovation: a comparison of Western and Bushman children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 126, 384–394. (doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.05.008)
Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. 2010 The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83. (doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X)
Unicef. 2010 Education in Serbia. See: https://www. unicef.org/ceecis/Serbia_2010.pdf(accessed 25 February 2017).
Statistical Office of Serbia. 2015 [The use of information and communication technologies in the Republic of Serbia]. See http://webrzs.stat.gov. rs/WebSite/public/PublicationView.aspx? pKey=41&p (accessed 25 February 2017).
Internet World Stats. 2015 France: Internet Usage Stats and Telecom Reports. See http://www. internetworldstats.com/eu/fr.htm(accessed 25 February 2017).
Beck SR, Williams C, Cutting N, Apperly IA, Chappell J. 2016 Individual differences in children’s innovative problem-solving are not predicted by divergent thinking or executive functions. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 371, 20150190. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2015.0190)
McGuigan N, Makinson J, Whiten A. 2011 From over-imitation to super-copying: adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 1–18. (doi:10.1348/000712610X493115)
IBM Corp. Released. 2012 IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 21.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.
Kenward B. 2012 Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 112, 195–207. (doi:10.1016/j.jecp. 2012.02.006)
McGuigan N. 2013 The influence of model status on the tendency of young children to over-imitate. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 116, 962–969. (doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.05.004)
Tomasello M. 2016 The ontogeny of cultural learning. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 8, 1–4. (doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.008)
Lillard A, Else-Quest N. 2006 The early years: evaluating Montessori education. Science 313, 1893–1894. (doi:10.1126/science.1132362)
Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H. 2005 In search of the uniquely human. Behav. Brain. Sci. 28, 721–727. (doi:10.1017/S0140525X05540123)
Southgate V, Chevallier C, Csibra G. 2009 Sensitivity to communicative relevance tells young children what to imitate. Dev. Sci 12, 1013–1019. (doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00861.x)
Terrier N, Bernard S, Mercier H, Clément F. 2016 Visual access trumps gender in 3- and 4-year-old children’s endorsement of testimony. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 146, 223–230. (doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016. 02.002)
McGuigan N. 2012 The role of transmission biases in the cultural diffusion of irrelevant actions. J. Comp. Psychol. 126, 150–160. (doi:10.1037/a002 5525)
Hanayama A, Mori K. 2011 Conformity of six-year-old children in the Asch experiment without using confederates. Psychology 2, 661–664. (doi:10.4236/psych.2011.27100)
Haun D, Tomasello M. 2011 Conformity to peer pressure in preschool children. Child Dev. 82, 1759–1767. (doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01666.x)
Cross CP, Brown GR, Morgan TJ, Laland KN. 2016 Gender differences in confidence influence patterns of conformity. Brit. J. Psychol. 108, 655–667 (doi:10.1111/bjop.12232)
Gredlein JM, Bjorklund DF. 2005 Gender differences in young children’s use of tools in a problem-solving task. Hum. Nat. 16, 211–232. (doi:10.1007/s12110-005-1004-5)
Koops K, Furuichi T, Hashimoto C, van Schaik CP. 2015 Gender differences in object manipulation in wild immature chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) and bonobos (Pan paniscus): preparation for tool use? PLoS ONE 10, e0139909. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0139909)
Gruber T, Clay Z, Zuberbühler K. 2010 A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage. Anim. Behav. 80, 1023–1033. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.09.005)
Frick A, Clément F, Gruber T. 2017 Data from: Evidence for a sex effect during overimitation: boys copy irrelevant modelled actions more than girls across cultures. Open Science Framework. (https://osf.io/2e86d)