This is an Original Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology on October 10th 2019, available at https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2019.1676881
All documents in ORBi are protected by a user license.
developmental language disorder; analogical reasoning; articulatory suppression; inhibition; phonological recoding
Abstract :
[en] Introduction. Analogical reasoning is a human ability of crucial importance in several domains of cognition, such as numerical abilities, social cognition, and language, and which is impaired in children with developmental language disorder (DLD). This impairment might be caused by their weaknesses in inhibition or by the inefficient use of phonological recoding.
Method. We compared children with DLD and age-matched children without language disorders in an A:B::C:D analogical task. We manipulated two variables: interference (versus no interference) was used to evaluate the impact of articulatory suppression on analogical performance, and distraction (versus no distractor) was used to test whether perceptual distractors (which compete with relational responses) diminish children’s performance.
Results. Contrary to expectations, articulatory suppression does not have a negative effect on analogical reasoning. In contrast, perceptual distractors have a detrimental impact on performance, and children with DLD are more impacted by perceptual distraction than their peers. Moreover, inhibition, as measured by a classical inhibition task, influences performance, but only for children with DLD.
Conclusion. The analogical reasoning impairment observed in DLD, therefore, seems to be related to perceptual distraction and inhibition rather than to phonological recoding. To conclude, this study investigates the analogical reasoning impairment observed in DLD and contributes to our understanding of the relationships between language, analogical reasoning, and executive functions.
Disciplines :
Theoretical & cognitive psychology
Author, co-author :
Krzemien, Magali ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Logopédie > Logopédie clinique
Thibaut, Jean-Pierre; Université de Bourgogne-Franche Comté > LEAD CNRS UMR 5022
Maillart, Christelle ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Logopédie > Logopédie clinique
Language :
English
Title :
How language and inhibition influence analogical reasoning in children with or without developmental language disorder?
Publication date :
2020
Journal title :
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology
scite shows how a scientific paper has been cited by providing the context of the citation, a classification describing whether it supports, mentions, or contrasts the cited claim, and a label indicating in which section the citation was made.
Bibliography
Baddeley, A., (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556–559.
Baddeley, A. D., & Larsen, J. D., (2007). The phonological loop unmasked? A comment on the evidence for a “perceptual-gestural” alternative. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(4),497–504.
Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M., (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14(6), 575–589.
Balthazar, C. H., (2003). The word length effect in children with language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(6), 487–505.
Bishop, D. V., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., Greenhalgh, T., & Catalise‐2 Consortium. (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(10), 1068–1080.
Botting, N., (2005). Non‐verbal cognitive development and language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(3), 317–326.
Bybee, J., (2010). Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Campoy, G., & Baddeley, A., (2008). Phonological and semantic strategies in immediate serial recall. Memory, 16(4), 329–340.
Christie, S., & Gentner, D., (2014). Language helps children succeed on a classic analogy task. Cognitive Science, 38(2), 383–397.
Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L., (2008). Uses and interpretations of non‐word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(1), 1–40.
Cohen, J., (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cuperus, J., Vugs, B., Scheper, A., & Hendriks, M., (2014). Executive function behaviours in children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 60(3), 132–143.
Dunn, L. M., Theriault-Whalen, C. M., & Dunn, L. M., (1993). Echelle de vocabulaire en images peabody. Toronto, Canada: Psychan.
Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A., (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: A dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 148–168.
Fatzer, S. T., & Roebers, C. M., (2012). Language and executive functions: The effect of articulatory suppression on executive functioning in children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13(4), 454–472.
Gentner, D., & Christie, S., (2010). Mutual bootstrapping between language and analogical processing. Language and Cognition, 2(2), 261–283.
Gentner, D., Simms, N., & Flusberg, S., (2009). Relational language helps children reason analogically. In Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1054–1059). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Cognitive Science Society.
Gentner, D., & Smith, L., (2012). Analogical reasoning. In V. S., Ramachandran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior (2nd ed., pp. 130–136). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
Geurten, M., Catale, C., & Meulemans, T., (2016). Involvement of executive functions in children’s metamemory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30(1), 70–80.
Hill, E. L., (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36(2), 149–171.
Hofstadter, D., & Sander, E., (2013). L’Analogie: Cœur de la pensée. Paris, France: Odile Jacob Sciences.
Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J., (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77(6), 1822–1841.
Kemény, F., & Lukács, Á., (2010). Impaired procedural learning in language impairment: Results from probabilistic categorization. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(3), 249–258.
Khomsi, A., (2001). ELO: Evaluation du langage oral. Paris, France: ECPS.
Kotovsky, L., & Gentner, D., (1996). Comparison and categorization in the development of relational similarity. Child Development, 67(6), 2797–2822.
Krzemien, M., Jemel, B., & Maillart, C., (2017). Analogical reasoning in children with specific language impairment: Evidence from a scene analogy task. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 31, 573–588.
Kuusisto, M. A., Nieminen, P. E., Helminen, M. T., & Kleemola, L., (2017). Executive and intellectual functioning in school‐aged children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 52(2),127–136.
Larsen, J. D.,&Baddeley, A., (2003). Disruption of verbal STM by irrelevant speech, articulatory suppression, and manual tapping: Do they have a common source? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A,56(8), 1249–1268.
Lecocq, P., (1996). L’ECOSSE: Une épreuve de compréhension syntaxico-sémantique. Villeneuve-d’Ascq, France: Presses Universitaires du Septentrion.
Leonard, L. B., (2014). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Leroy, S., Maillart, C., & Parisse, C., (2014). Analogical mapping across modalities in children with specific language impairment (SLI). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(9), 2158–2171.
Leroy, S., Parisse, C., & Maillart, C., (2012). Analogical reasoning in children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 26(4), 380–395.
Leroy, S., Parisse, C., & Maillart, C., (2014). Le manque de généralisation chez les enfants dysphasiques: Une étude longitudinale. ANAE, 131, 1–9.
Lidstone, J. S., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C., (2010). The roles of private speech and inner speech in planning during middle childhood: Evidence from a dual task paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(4), 438–451.
Lidstone, J. S., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C., (2012). Verbal mediation of cognition in children with specific language impairment. Development and Psychopathology, 24(2), 651–660.
Logie, R. H., Della Sala, S., Wynn, V., & Baddeley, A. D., (2000). Visual similarity effects in immediate verbal serial recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 53(3), 626–646.
Lum, J. A., & Conti-Ramsden, G., (2013). Long-term memory: A review and meta-analysis of studies of declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Topics in Language Disorders, 33(4), 282–297.
Maguire, M. J., Schneider, J. M., Middleton, A. E., Ralph, Y., Lopez, M., Ackerman, R. A., & Abel, A. D., (2018). Vocabulary knowledge mediates the link between socioeconomic status and word learning in grade school. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 679–695.
Masterson, J. J., Evans, L. H., & Aloia, M., (1993). Verbal analogical reasoning in children with language-learning disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 76–82.
Mussweiler, T., & Epstude, K., (2009). Relatively fast! Efficiency advantages of comparative thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(1), 1–21.
Niemi, J., Gundersen, H., Leppäsaari, T., & Hugdahl, K., (2003). Speech lateralization and attention/executive functions in a Finnish family with specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(4), 457–464.
Nippold, M. A., Erskine, B. J., & Freed, D. B., (1988). Proportional and functional analogical reasoning in normal and language-impaired children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 53, 440–448.
Norris, D., Butterfield, S., Hall, J., & Page, M. P., (2018). Phonological recoding under articulatory suppression. Memory & Cognition, 46(2), 173–180.
Otsuka, Y., & Osaka, N., (2015). High-performers use the phonological loop less to process mental arithmetic during working memory tasks.The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(5),878–886.
Poloczek, S.,Henry, L. A.,Messer, D. J., & Büttner, G., (2019).Do children use different forms of verbal rehearsal in serial picture recall tasks? A multi-method study. Memory, 27,758–771.
Psychology Software Tools, Inc. (2012). E-Prime 2.0. Retrieved from http://www.pstnet.com
R Development Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Ramus, F., Marshall, C. R., Rosen, S., & van der Lely, H. K., (2013). Phonological deficits in specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: Towards a multidimensional model. Brain, 136(2), 630–645.
Rao, K. V., & Baddeley, A., (2013). Raven’s matrices and working memory: A dual-task approach. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(10), 1881–1887.
Richland, L. E.,Morrison, R. G., & Holyoak, K. J., (2006). Children’s development of analogical reasoning: Insights from scene analogy problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94(3), 249–273.
Stigler, J. W., Lee, S. Y., & Stevenson, H. W., (1986). Digit memory in Chinese and English: Evidence for a temporally limited store. Cognition, 23(1), 1–20.
Sullivan, J., & Barner, D., (2013). How are number words mapped to approximate magnitudes? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(2), 389–402.
Thibaut, J.-P., French, R., & Vezneva, M., (2010). The development of analogy making in children: Cognitive load and executive functions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(1), 1–19.
Thibaut, J. P., French, R. M., Vezneva, M., Gérard, Y., & Glady, Y., (2011). Semantic analogies by young children: Testing the role of inhibition. In B., Kokinov, A., Karmiloff-Smith, & N. J., Nersessian (Eds.), European perspectives on cognitive science (pp. 136–140). Sofia, Bulgaria: New Bulgarian University Press.
Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Xuyang, Z., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M., (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260.
Vezneva, M., (2011). Développement du raisonnement analogique: Rôle de la composante exécutive d’inhibition (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France.
Vugs, B., Hendriks, M. P. H., Cuperus, J., & Knoors, H., (2017). Developmental associations between working memory and language in children with specific language impairment: A longitudinal study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(11), 2384–2394.
Waltz, J. A., Lau, A., Grewal, S. K., & Holyoak, K. J., (2000). The role of working memory in analogical mapping. Memory & Cognition, 28(7), 1205–1212.
Wechsler, D., & Naglieri, J., (2009). Echelle non verbale d’intelligence de Wechsler. Montreuil, France: ECPA.
Yang, H.-C., & Gray, S., (2017). Executive function in preschoolers with primary language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 379–392.
Zelaznik, H. N., & Goffman, L., (2010). Generalized motor abilities and timing behavior in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(2), 383–393.
This website uses cookies to improve user experience. Read more
Save & Close
Accept all
Decline all
Show detailsHide details
Cookie declaration
About cookies
Strictly necessary
Performance
Strictly necessary cookies allow core website functionality such as user login and account management. The website cannot be used properly without strictly necessary cookies.
This cookie is used by Cookie-Script.com service to remember visitor cookie consent preferences. It is necessary for Cookie-Script.com cookie banner to work properly.
Performance cookies are used to see how visitors use the website, eg. analytics cookies. Those cookies cannot be used to directly identify a certain visitor.
Used to store the attribution information, the referrer initially used to visit the website
Cookies are small text files that are placed on your computer by websites that you visit. Websites use cookies to help users navigate efficiently and perform certain functions. Cookies that are required for the website to operate properly are allowed to be set without your permission. All other cookies need to be approved before they can be set in the browser.
You can change your consent to cookie usage at any time on our Privacy Policy page.