visual short-term memory; processing capacities; specific language impairment
Abstract :
[en] Many studies have assessed visual short-term memory (VSTM) abilities in children with specific language impairment (SLI), with contrasting results: some studies observed preserved VSTM capacities while others reported impaired VSTM. The present study explores the hypothesis that the complexity of the visual information to be encoded and stored might underlie these discrepancies. Four VSTM conditions were administered to a group of 15 children with SLI, as well as to two groups of typically developing children, matched for chronological age and for VSTM capacity for visually simple stimuli, respectively. The stimuli to be remembered varied in their visual similarity and in the number of their visual features. Across the four VSTM conditions, children with SLI showed significantly reduced performance relative to an age-matched control group, and they were more strongly affected by visual similarity and number of features when compared to a control group matched for VSTM capacity for visually simple stimuli. The present results support the hypothesis that stimulus complexity is a determining factor of the poor VSTM performances in children with SLI.
scite shows how a scientific paper has been cited by providing the context of the citation, a classification describing whether it supports, mentions, or contrasts the cited claim, and a label indicating in which section the citation was made.
Bibliography
Alloway, T.P., & Archibald, L. (2008). Working memory and learning in children with developmental coordination disorder and specific language impairment. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(3), 251-262.
Alloway, T.P., Rajendran, G., & Archibald, L. (2009). Working memory in children with developmental disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(4), 372-382.
Alvarez, G., & Cavanagh, P. (2004). The capacity of visual short term memory is set both by visual information load and by number of objects. Psychological Science, 15(2), 106-111.
Andrade, J., Kemps, E., Werniers, Y., May, J., & Szmalec, A. (2002). Insensitivity of visual short-term memory to irrelevant visual information. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 3(3), 753-774.
Archibald, L.M., & Gathercole, S.E. (2006). Visuospatial immediate memory on specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(2), 265-277.
Archibald, L.M., & Gathercole, S.E. (2007). The complexities of complex memory span: Storage and processing deficits in specific language impairment. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(2), 177-194.
Avons, S., & Mason, A. (1999). Effects of visual similarity on serial report and item recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 1(1), 217-240.
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(3), 189-208.
Bavin, E.L., Wilson, P.H., Maruff, P., & Sleeman, F. (2005). Spatiovisual memory of children with specific language impairment: Evidence for generalized processing problems. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 40(3), 319-332.
Bishop, D. (1997). Cognitive neuropsychology and developmental disorders: Uncomfortable bedfellows. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 4(4), 899-923.
Bishop, D.V., & Norbury, C.F. (2005). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology 2: Response inhibition. Autism, 9(1), 29-43.
Della Sala, S., & Logie, R. (2002). Neuropsychological impairments of visual ans spatial working memory. In A. Baddeley, M.D. Kopelman, & B.A. Wilson (Eds.), The handbook of memory disorders. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Dunn, L., Thériault-Whalen, C., & Dunn, L. (1993). Échelle de Vocabulaire en Images Peabody. Toronto: Psycan.
Edwards, J., & Munson, B. (2009). Speech perception and production in child language disordersHandbook of child language disorders (pp. 216-231). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Eng, H.Y., Chen, D., & Jiang, Y. (2005). Visual working memory for simple and complex visual stimuli. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 1127-1133.
Evans, J.L. (2002). Variability in comprehension strategy use in children with SLI: A dynamical systems account. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 37(2), 95-116.
Fazio, B.B. (1998). The effect of presentation rate on serial memory in young children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(6), 1375-1383.
Finneran, D.A., Francis, A.L., & Leonard, L.B. (2009). Sustained attention in children with specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(4), 915-929.
Forsythe, A. (2009). Visual complexity: Is that all there is? Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Engineering Psychology and Cognitive Ergonomics, HCII 2009, 5639, pp. 158-166.
Gathercole, S.E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(4), 513-543.
Hick, R., Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2005). Cognitive abilities in children with specific language impairment: Consideration of visuo-spatial skills. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 40(2), 137-149.
Hoffman, L.M., & Gillam, R.B. (2004). Verbal and spatial information processing constraints in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(1), 114-125.
Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77(6), 1822-1841.
Khomsi, A. (2001). Evaluation du langage oral. Paris: ECPS.
Lecocq, P. (1996). Epreuve de Compréhension Syntaxico- Sémantique. Villeneuve d'Ascq Presses universitaires du Septentrion.
Leonard, L.B., Weismer, S.E., Miller, C.A., Francis, D.J., Tomblin, J., & Kail, R.V. (2007). Speed of processing, working memory, and language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(2), 408-428.
Lin, P.-H., & Luck, S.J. (2008). The influence of similarity on visual working memory representations. Visual Cognition, 17(3), 356-372.
Logie, R.H., Della Sala, S., Wynn, V., & Baddeley, A.D. (2000). Visual similarity effects in immediate verbal serial recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 3(3), 626-646.
Lum, J.A., Conti-Ramsden, G., & Lindell, A.K. (2007). The attentional blink reveals sluggish attentional shifting in adolescents with specific language impairment. Brain and Cognition, 63(3), 287-295.
Luria, R., Sessa, P., Gotler, A., Jolicoeur, P., & DellAcqua, R. (2010). Visual short-term memory capacity for simple and complex objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(3), 496-512.
Majerus, S., D'Argembeau, A., Perez, T., Belayachi, S., Van der Linden, M., Collette, F.,yMaquet, P. (2010). The commonality of neural networks for verbal and visual short-term memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(11), 2570-2593.
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Van Der Linden, M., Albouy, G., Salmon, E., Sterpenich, V., y Maquet, P. (2006). The left intraparietal sulcu and verbal short-term memory: Focus of attention or serial order ? Neuroimage, 32, 880-891.
Marton, K. (2006). Commentaries: Do nonword repetition errors in children with specific language impairment reflect a weakness in an unidentified skill specific to nonword repetition or a deficit in simultaneous processing? [Comment/Reply]. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(4), 569-573.
Marton, K., & Schwartz, R.G. (2003). Working memory capacity and language processes in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(5), 1138-1153.
Mate, J., & Baques, J. (2009). Visual similarity at encoding and retrieval in an item recognition task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(7), 1277-1284.
Miller, C.A., Kail, R., Leonard, L.B., & Tomblin, J. (2001). Speed of processing in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(2), 416-433.
Montgomery, J.W. (2005). Effects of input rate and age on the realtime language processing of children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 40(2), 171-188.
Montgomery, J.W. (2008). Role of auditory attention in the realtime processing of simple grammar by children with specific language impairment: A preliminary investigation. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(5), 499-527.
Nairne, J.S., & Kelley, M.R. (2004). Separating item and order information through process dissociation. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(2), 113-133.
Nickisch, A., & von Kries, R. (2009). Short-term memory (STM) constraints in children with specific language impairment (SLI): Are there differences between receptive and expressive SLI? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(3), 578-595.
Noterdaeme, M., Amorosa, H., Mildenberger, K., Sitter, S., & Minow, F. (2001). Evaluation of attention problems in children with autism and children with a specific language disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 10(1), 58-66.
Poirier, M., & Saint-Aubin, J. (1996). Immediate serial recall, word frequency, item identity and item position. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale, 50(4), 408-412.
Poirier, M., Saint-Aubin, J., Musselwhite, K., Mohanadas, T., & Mahammed, G. (2007). Visual similarity effects on short-term memory for order: The case of verbally labeled pictorial stimuli. Memory & Cognition, 35(4), 711-723.
Poncelet, M., & van der Linden, M. (2003). Assessing the phonological store of working memory: Elaboration of a nonword repetition test for a French-speaking population. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 13(3), 377-407.
Powell, R., & Bishop, D. (1992). Clumsiness and perceptual problems in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 34(9), 755-765.
Romani, C., Ward, J., & Olson, A. (1999). Developmental surface dysgraphia: What is the underlying cognitive impairment? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 1(1), 97-128.
Schneider, W., Eschmann, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user's guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools.
Schul, R., Stiles, J., Wulfeck, B., & Townsend, J. (2004). How 'generalized' is the 'slowed processing' in SLI? The case of visuospatial attentional orienting. Neuropsychologia, 42(5), 661-671.
Tallal, P., Stark, R., Kallman, C., & Mellits, D. (1981). A reexamination of some nonverbal perceptual abilities of languageimpaired and normal children as a function of age and sensory modality. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 24(3), 351-357.
Wechsler, D. (2005). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children 4e Edition. Paris: ECPA.
Weismer, S.E., & Hesketh, L.J. (1996). Lexical learning by children with specific language impairment: Effects of linguistic input presented at varying speaking rates. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 39(1), 177-190.
Weismer, S.E., Plante, E., Jones, M., & Tomblin, J. (2005). A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of verbal working memory in adolescents with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(2), 405-425.
Windsor, J., & Kohnert, K. (2004). The search for common ground: Part I. Lexical performance by linguistically diverse learners. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(4), 877-890.
Wood, J.N. (2011). A core knowledge architecture of visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(2), 357-381.
Xu, Y. (2007). The role of the superior intraparietal sulcus in supporting visual short-term memory for multifeature objects. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(43), 11676-11686.
This website uses cookies to improve user experience. Read more
Save & Close
Accept all
Decline all
Show detailsHide details
Cookie declaration
About cookies
Strictly necessary
Performance
Strictly necessary cookies allow core website functionality such as user login and account management. The website cannot be used properly without strictly necessary cookies.
This cookie is used by Cookie-Script.com service to remember visitor cookie consent preferences. It is necessary for Cookie-Script.com cookie banner to work properly.
Performance cookies are used to see how visitors use the website, eg. analytics cookies. Those cookies cannot be used to directly identify a certain visitor.
Used to store the attribution information, the referrer initially used to visit the website
Cookies are small text files that are placed on your computer by websites that you visit. Websites use cookies to help users navigate efficiently and perform certain functions. Cookies that are required for the website to operate properly are allowed to be set without your permission. All other cookies need to be approved before they can be set in the browser.
You can change your consent to cookie usage at any time on our Privacy Policy page.