Adult; Female; Humans; Male; Voice Quality; Memory; Pitch Perception; Speech Perception; Categorization; Earwitness testimony; Memory distortions; Voice memory; Experimental and Cognitive Psychology; Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous); Psychology (all); General Psychology; General Medicine
Abstract :
[en] Corneille, Huart, Becquart, & Brédart (2004) found that people remember ambiguous race faces as closer to a race prototype than they actually are. In three studies, we examined whether this memory bias generalizes to voice memory. In Studies 1 and 2, participants listened to synthesized male and female speech samples (high, moderate, or low pitch) and were asked to identify a voice target when paired against distracters higher or lower in pitch. The results showed that pitch distortions occurred, with the pattern consistent with assimilation toward low and high ends of the pitch continuum. Study 3 replicated this result with a wider voice pitch range. The results parallel those of Corneille et al. (2004). The implications of this work are discussed.
Disciplines :
Social, industrial & organizational psychology
Author, co-author :
Stern, Steven E; Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh at Johnstown 15904, USA. sstern@pitt.edu
Mullennix, John W; University of Pittsburgh, Johnstown, United States
Corneille, Olivier; Catholic University of Louvain, Louvain la Neuve, Belgium
Huart, Johanne ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Psychologie > Psychologie sociale ; Catholic University of Louvain, Louvain la Neuve, Belgium
Allport, G.W. (1954/1979). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books.
Bartlett, F.C. (1932). Remembering: An experimental and social study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Boë, L.-J., Contini, M., & Rakotofiringa, H. (1975). Étude statistique de la fréquence laryngienne [Statistical study of laryngeal frequencies]. Phonetica, 32, 1-23.
Brewer, W.F., & Nakamura, G. V. (1984). The nature and functions of schemas. In R.S. Wyer & T.K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 119-160). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bull, R., & Clifford, B.R. (1984). Earwitness voice recognition accuracy. In G.L. Wells & E.F. Loftus (Eds.) Eyewitness testimony: Psychological perspectives. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Chevrie-Muller, C., & Gremy, F. (1967). Contribution a 1'éstablissement de quelques constantes physiologiques de la voix parlée de l'adulte [Contribution to the establishment of various physiologic parameters of the speaking voice of adults]. Journal Français d'Oto-Rhino-Laryngologie, 16, 433-455.
Cook, S., & Wilding, J. (1997). Earwitness testimony 2: Voices, faces and context. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 527-541.
Cook, S., & Wilding, J. (2001). Earwitness testimony: Effects of exposure and attention on the face overshadowing effect. British Journal of Psychology, 92,617-629.
Corneille, O., Huart, J., Becquart, E., & Brédart, S. (2004). When memory shifts toward more typical category exemplars: Accentuation effects in the recollection of ethnically ambiguous faces. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 236-250.
Deffenbacher, K.A., Cross, J.F., Handkins, R.E., Chance, J.E., Goldstein, A.G., Hammersly, R. et al. (1989). Relevance of voice identification research to criteria for evaluating reliability of an identification. The Journal of Psychology, 123, 109-119.
Estes, W.K. (1994). Classification and cognition. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 500-549.
Fiedler, K., Armbruster, T., Nickel, S., Walther, E., & Asbeck, J. (1996). Constructive biases in social judgment: Experiments on the self-verification of question contents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 861-873.
Fiedler, K., Walther, E., Armbruster, T., Fay, D., & Naumann, U. (1996). Do you really know what you have seen? Intrusion errors and presuppositions effects on constructive memory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 484-511.
Flanagan, J.L. (1972). Speech analysis, synthesis and perception. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Harnad, S. (1987). Introduction: psychophysical and cognitive aspects of categorical perception: A critical overview. In S. Harnad (Ed.), Categorical perception: The groundwork of cognition (pp. 1-25). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hollien, H. (1974). On vocal registers. Journal of Phonetics, 2, 125-143.
Huart, J., Cornielle, O., & Becquart, E. (2005). Face-based categorization, context-based categorization, and distortions in the recollection of gender ambiguous faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 598-608.
Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L.V., & Duncan, S. (1991). Categories and particulars: Prototype effects in estimating spatial location. Psychological Review, 98, 352-376.
Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L.V., & Vevea, J.L. (2000). Why do categories affect stimulus judgment? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 220-241.
Klatt, D.H., & Klatt, L.C. (1990). Analysis, synthesis, and perception of voice quality variations among female and male talkers. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 87, 820-857.
Koriat, A., Goldsmith, M., & Pansky, A. (2000). Toward a psychology of memory accuracy. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 481-537.
Kreiman, J., & Papcun, G. (1991). Comparing discrimination and recognition of unfamiliar voices. Speech Communication, 10, 265-275.
Kuhl, P. (1991). Human adults and human infants show a "perceptual magnet effect" for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not. Perception and Psychophysics, 50, 93-107.
Lamberts, K. (1996). Exemplar models and prototype effects in similarity-based categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1503-1507.
Loftus, E.F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48, 518-537.
Loftus, E.F., & Palmer, J.P. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585-589.
McAllister, H.A., Blair, M.J., Cerone, L.G., & Laurent, M.J. (2000). Multimedia mug books: How multi should the media bel Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 277-291.
Medin, D.L., & Schaffer, M.M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207-238.
Melara, R.D., & DeWitt-Rickards, T.S., & O'Brien, T.P. (1989). Enhancing lineup identification accuracy: Two codes are better than one. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 706-713.
Minda, J.P., & Smith, J.D. (2002). Comparing prototype-based and exemplar-based accounts of category learning and attentional allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 275-292.
Nosofsky, R.M. (1992). Exemplars, prototypes, and similarity rules. In A.F. Healy, S.M. Kosslyn, & R.M. Shiffrin (Eds.), From learning theory to connectionist theory: Essays in honor of William K. Estes (pp. 149-167). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Nosofsky, R.M., & Zaki, S.R. (2002). Exemplar and prototype models revisited: Response strategies, selective attention, and stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 924-940.
Olsson, N., Juslin, P., & Winman, A. (1998). Realism of confidence in earwitness versus eyewitness identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4, 101-118.
Palmeri, T.J., & Nosofsky, R.M. (2001). Central tendencies, extreme points, and prototype enhancement effects in ill-defined perceptual categorization. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 197-235.
Papcun, G., Kreiman, J., & Davis, A. (1989). Long-term memory for unfamiliar voices. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 85, 913-925.
Peterson, G.E., & Barney, H.L. (1952). Control methods used in a study of the vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 24, 175-184.
Posner, M.I., & Keele, S.W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 353-363.
Roediger, H.L., & McDermott, K.B. (2000). Distortions in memory. In E.E. Tulving & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.). The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 149-162). New York: Oxford University Press.
Smith, J.D. (2002). Exemplar theory's predicted typicality gradient can be tested and disconfirmed. Psychological Science, 13, 437-442.
Smith, J.D. (2005). Wanted: A new psychology of exemplars. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 47-53.
Spisak, B., Mullennix, J.W., Moro, K., Will, J., & Farnsworth, L.M. (2002, June). Typicality ratings of male and female voices. Paper presented at the 143rd Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America, Pittsburgh, PA.
Stern, S.E., Corneille, O., Huart, J., & Mullennix, J.W. (2004, May). Memory accentuation effects for the pitch and speed of speech. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Belgian Psychological Society, Free University of Brussels, Brussels, Belgium.
Storms, G., Boeck, P.D., and Ruts, W. (2000). Prototype and exemplar-based information in natural language categories. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 51-73.
Tajfel, H., & Wilkes, A.L. (1963). Classification and quantitative judgment. British Journal of Psychology, 54, 101-114.
Valentine, T. (1991). A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion and race in face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, 161-204.
Walley, A.C., & Sloane, M.E. (2001). The perceptual magnet effect: A review of empirical findings and theoretical implications. In F. Columbus (Ed.), Advances in psychology research, Vol., 4 (pp. 65-92). Hauppauge, NY: Nova.
Yarmey, A.D., Yarmey, A.L., Yarmey, M.J., & Parliament, L. (2001). Commonsense beliefs and the identification of familiar voices. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 283-299.
Zaki, S.R., Nosofsky, R.M., Stanton, R.D., & Cohen, A.L. (2003). Prototype and exemplar accounts of category learning and attentional allocation: A reassessment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2, 1160-1173.