How Do Spelling, Handwriting Speed, and Handwriting Quality Develop During Primary School? Cross-Classified Growth Curve Analysis of Children's Writing Development.
Gosse, Claire; Parmentier, Michaël; Van Reybroeck, Marie
[en] Aim: Longitudinal studies are rare in the field of writing research, and little is known about the concurrent development of the two transcription skills: spelling and handwriting. This study was designed to provide a comprehensive picture of the development and the longitudinal relations between spelling, handwriting speed, and handwriting quality at the word level. Method: Over a period of 3 years (coh1: Grades 2-4; coh2: Grades 3-5), 117 French-speaking children were assessed on a single-word dictation task. At each testing time, measures of spelling accuracy, handwriting speed, and handwriting quality were collected on 40 words. Words varied in both orthographic and graphic complexity, making it possible to investigate the influence of these levels of complexity on transcription abilities. Results: Linear growth analyses using cross-classified Bayesian structural equation modeling (CC-BSEM) revealed that spelling and speed continued to improve until Grade 5, while handwriting quality reached an early plateau in Grade 2. In the younger cohort, graphic complexity had a significant influence on the pace of development of handwriting speed and on spelling and handwriting quality performance in Grade 2. In the older cohort, a positive relation between spelling and speed and a negative relation between handwriting speed and handwriting quality were found, indicating that fast handwriting is associated with high spelling ability and that fast handwriting is detrimental to handwriting quality. By providing a better understanding of writing development, this study yields innovative findings not only regarding the development of transcription skills but also regarding how spelling, handwriting speed, and handwriting quality can influence each other's performance throughout primary school.
Research Center/Unit :
HEC Recherche. Changing workplace and strategic HRM - ULiège LENTIC - Laboratoire d'Études sur les Nouvelles Formes de Travail, l'Innovation et le Changement - ULiège
Disciplines :
Theoretical & cognitive psychology
Author, co-author :
Gosse, Claire; Psychological Sciences Research Institute (IPSY), Catholic University of Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
Parmentier, Michaël ; Université de Liège - ULiège > HEC Liège Research > HEC Liège Research: Changing workplace and strategic HRM ; Psychological Sciences Research Institute (IPSY), Catholic University of Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
Van Reybroeck, Marie; Psychological Sciences Research Institute (IPSY), Catholic University of Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
Language :
English
Title :
How Do Spelling, Handwriting Speed, and Handwriting Quality Develop During Primary School? Cross-Classified Growth Curve Analysis of Children's Writing Development.
This study benefitted from financial support from Fondation Louvain (Belgium) and Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique— FNRS (Belgium; Grant 1.A.690.18F).We would like to offer special thanks to our collaborators from the ICTM, S. Carbonnelle and Prof. C. De Vleeschouwer, for data collection and analysis. We thank Mika?l De Clercq and Elizabeth Olivier for their advice on statistical analysis, and Elise Blampain and Margot De Rom for their comments on the manuscript. We are grateful to the children and their parents, the directors and the teachers who took part in the study. Funding. This study benefitted from financial support from Fondation Louvain (Belgium) and Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique?FNRS (Belgium; Grant 1.A.690.18F).
Abbott R. D., Berninger V. W., Fayol M., (2010). Longitudinal relationships of levels of language in writing and between writing and reading in grades 1 to 7. J. Educ. Psychol. 102, 281–298. 10.1037/a0019318
Alamargot D., Morin M.-F., Simard-Dupuis E., (2020). Handwriting delay in dyslexia: children at the end of primary school still make numerous short pauses when producing letters. J. Learn. Disabil. 53, 163–175. 10.1177/00222194209037032036738
Alves R. A., Limpo T., (2015). Progress in written language bursts, pauses, transcription, and written composition across schooling. Scientif. Stud. Read. 19, 374–391. 10.1080/10888438.2015.1059838
Arfé B., Corato F., Pizzocaro E., Merella A., (2020). The effects of script and orthographic complexity on the handwriting and spelling performance of children with dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 53, 96–108. 10.1177/002221941989284531823679
Baayen R. H., Davidson D. J., Bates D. M., (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. J. Mem. Lang. 59, 390–412. 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.00533635157
Bara F., Morin M. F., (2013). Does the handwriting style learned in first grade determine the style used in the fourth and fifth grades and influence handwriting speed and quality? A comparison between French and Quebec children. Psychol. Sch. 50, 601–617. 10.1002/pits.21691
Barnett A. L., Connelly V., Miller B., (2019). The interaction of reading, spelling, and handwriting difficulties with writing development. J. Learn. Disabil. 53, 92–95. 10.1177/002221941989456531847693
Barnett A. L., Prunty M., Rosenblum S., (2018). Development of the handwriting legibility scale (HLS): a preliminary examination of reliability and validity. Res. Dev. Disabil. 72, 240–247. 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.11.01329223112
Beneath B., Boutard C., Alberti C., (2006). Chronosdictées: Outils D'évaluation des Performances Orthographiques Avec et Sans Contraintes Temporelles: du CE1 à la Troisième. Paris: Ortho Edition.
Blampain E., Gosse C., Van Reybroeck M., (2021). Copying skills in children with and without dyslexia. Read. Writ. 1–27, 859–885. 10.1007/s11145-020-10094-6
Bosga-Stork I. M., Bosga J., Ellis J. L., Meulenbroek R. G., (2016). Developing interactions between language and motor skills in the first three years of formal handwriting education. Br. J. Educ. Soc. Behav. Sci. 12, 1–13. 10.9734/bjesbs/2016/20703
Browne W. J., Draper D., (2006). A comparison of Bayesian and likelihood-based methods for fitting multilevel models. Bayesian Anal. 1, 473–514. 10.1214/06-ba117
Camacho A., Alves R. A., Boscolo P., (2021). Writing motivation in school: a systematic review of empirical research in the early twenty-first century. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 33, 213–247. 10.1007/s10648-020-09530-4
Caravolas M., Downing C., Hadden C. L., Wynne C., (2020). Handwriting legibility and its relationship to spelling ability and age: evidence from Monolingual and Bilingual children. Front. Psychol. 11:1097. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.0109732581945
Casalis S., Deacon S. H., Pacton S., (2011). How specific is the connection between morphological awareness and spelling? A study of French children. Appl. Psycholinguist. 32, 499–511. 10.1017/s014271641100018x
Charles M., Soppelsa R., Albaret J.-M., (2004). BHK: Échelle D'évaluation Rapide de L'écriture Chez L'enfant. Paris: ECPA.
Chartrel E., Vinter A., (2004). L'écriture: une activité longue et complexe à acquérir [Writing acquisition: a long, complex process]. Approch. Neuropsychol. Apprentis. l'Enfant 16, 174–180.
Deacon S. H., Bryant P., (2006). This turnip's not for turning: Children's morphological awareness and their use of root morphemes in spelling. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 24, 567–575. 10.1348/026151005X50834
Delattre M., Bonin P., Barry C., (2006). Written spelling to dictation: sound-to-spelling regularity affects both writing latencies and durations. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 32, 1330–1340. 10.1037/0278-7393.32.6.133017087587
Di Brina C., Averna R., Rampoldi P., Rossetti S., Penge R., (2018). Reading and writing skills in children with specific learning disabilities with and without developmental coordination disorder. Motor Control 22, 391–405. 10.1123/mc.2016-000629488824
Downing C., Caravolas M., (2020). Prevalence and cognitive profiles of children with comorbid literacy and motor disorders. Front. Psychol. 11:3580. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.57358033362640
Dunn L. M., Dunn L. M., Theriault-Whalen C., (1993). EVIP: Échelle de Vocabulaire en Images Peabody. Toronto, ON: Pearson Canada Assessment.
Enders C. K., (2010). Applied Missing Data Analysis. New York: Guilford Press.
Feder K. P., Majnemer A., (2007). Handwriting development, competency, and intervention. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49, 312–317. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00312.x
Gill J., (2014). Bayesian Methods: A Social and Behavioral Sciences Approach (20). Hoboken, NJ: CRC Press.
Gill J., (2015). Bayesian Methods: A Social and Behavioral Sciences Approach, 3rd Edn. Taylor & Francis. 10.1198/000313008x370915
Gosse C., Carbonnelle S., de Vleeschouwer C., Van Reybroeck M., (2018). Specifying the graphic characteristics of words that influence children's handwriting. Read. Writ. 31, 1181–1207. 10.1007/s11145-018-9834-9
Gosse C., Van Reybroeck M., (2020). Do children with dyslexia present a handwriting deficit? Impact of word orthographic and graphic complexity on handwriting and spelling performance. Res. Dev. Disabil. 10.1016/j.ridd.2019.10355331841819
Graham S., (2010). Want to improve children's writing? The Education Digest 76, 49–55.
Graham S., (2018). Handwriting instruction: a commentary on five studies. Read. Writ. Interdiscipl. J. 31, 1367–1377. 10.1007/s11145-018-9854-5
Graham S., Berninger V., Weintraub N., Schafer W., (1998). Development of handwriting speed and legibility in grades 1–9. J. Educ. Res. 92, 42–52. 10.1080/00220679809597574
Graham S., Harris K. R., Mason L., Fink-Chorzempa B., Moran S., Saddler B., (2008). How do primary grade teachers teach handwriting? A national survey. Read. Writ. Interdiscipl. J. 21, 49–69. 10.1007/s11145-007-9064-z
Graham S., Struck M., Santoro J., Berninger V. W., (2006). Dimensions of good and poor handwriting legibility in first and second graders: motor programs, visual-spatial arrangement, and letter formation parameter setting. Dev. Neuropsychol. 29, 43–60. 10.1207/s15326942dn2901_416390288
Hayes J. R., (2006). New directions in writing theory. Handb. Writ. Res. 2, 28–40.
Jones D., Christensen C. A., (1999). Relationship between automaticity in handwriting and students' ability to generate written text. J. Educ. Psychol. 91, 44–49. 10.1037/0022-0663.91.1.44
Judd C. M., Westfall J., Kenny D. A., (2012). Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: a new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 103, 54–69. 10.1037/a002834722612667
Kandel S., Perret C., (2015). How does the interaction between spelling and motor processes build up during writing acquisition? Cognition 136, 325–336. 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.01425525970
Karlsdottir R., Stefansson T., (2002). Problems in developing functional handwriting. Percept. Mot. Skills 94, 623–662. 10.2466/pms.2002.94.2.623
Khomsi A., (1999). Lecture en une Minute (LUM). LMC-R: Epreuve D'évaluation de la.
Kline R. B., (2015). Principles and Practices of Structural Equation Modelling, 4th Edn. The Guilford Press.
Martinet C., Valdois S., Fayol M., (2004). Lexical orthographic knowledge develops from the beginning of literacy acquisition. Cognition 91, B11–B22. 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.09.00214738774
McCutchen D., (1996). A capacity theory of writing: working memory in composition. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 8, 299–325. 10.1007/BF01464076
Medwell J., Wray D., (2007). Handwriting: what do we know and what do we need to know? Literacy 41, 10–15. 10.1111/j.1467-9345.2007.00453.x
Morin M.-F., Lavoie N., Montésinos-Gelet I., (2012). The effects of manuscript, cursive or manuscript/cursive styles on writing development in Grade 2. Lang. Liter. 14, 110–124. 10.20360/g21s3v
Muthén B., Asparouhov T., (2012). Bayesian structural equation modeling: a more flexible representation of substantive theory. Psychol. Methods 17, 313–335. 10.1037/a002680222962886
Nestler S., Back M. D., (2017). Using cross-classified structural equation models to examine the accuracy of personality judgments. Psychometrika 82, 475–497. 10.1007/s11336-015-9485-626630991
New B., Pallier C., Brysbaert M., Ferrand L., (2004). Lexique 2: a new French lexical database. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 36, 516–524. 10.3758/bf0319559815641440
Newman D. A., (2014). Missing data: five practical guidelines. Organ. Res. Methods 17, 372–411. 10.1177/1094428114548590
Overvelde A., Hulstijn W., (2011). Handwriting development in grade 2 and grade 3 primary school children with normal, at risk, or dysgraphic characteristics. Res. Dev. Disabil. 32, 540–548. 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.12.02721269805
Pacton S., Perruchet P., Fayol M., Cleeremans A., (2001). Implicit learning out of the lab: the case of orthographic regularities. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130, 401–426. 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.40111561917
Palmis S., Velay J.-L., Fabiani E., Nazarian B., Anton J.-L., Habib M., et al. (2019). The impact of spelling regularity on handwriting production: a coupled fMRI and kinematics study. Cortex 113, 111–127. 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.11.02430640140
Pontart V., Bidet-Ildei C., Lambert E., Morisset P., Flouret L., Alamargot D., (2013). Influence of handwriting skills during spelling in primary and lower secondary grades. Front. Psychol. 4:818. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.0081824204357
Pothier B., Pothier P., (2003). Échelle d'acquisition en orthographe lexicale (EOLE). [Acquisition Scale for Lexical Spelling]. Paris: Retz.
Prunty M., Barnett A. L., (2017). Understanding handwriting difficulties: a comparison of children with and without motor impairment. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 34, 205–218. 10.1080/02643294.2017.137663028948860
Salas N., Caravolas M., (2019). Dimensionality of early writing in English and Spanish. J. Literacy Res. 51, 272–292. 10.1177/1086296X19858146
Sénéchal M., (2000). Morphological effects in children's spelling of French words. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 54:76. 10881392
Sprenger-Charolles L., Siegel L. S., Béchennec D., Serniclaes W., (2003). Development of phonological and orthographic processing in reading aloud, in silent reading, and in spelling: a four-year longitudinal study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 84, 194–217. 10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00024-912706384
Sprenger-Charolles L., Siegel L. S., Bonnet P., (1998). Reading and spelling acquisition in French: the role of phonological mediation and orthographic factors. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 68, 134–165. 10.1006/jecp.1997.24229503649
Sumner E., Connelly V., Barnett A. L., (2013). Children with dyslexia are slow writers because they pause more often and not because they are slow at handwriting execution. Read. Writ. 26, 991–1008. 10.1007/s11145-012-9403-6
Sumner E., Connelly V., Barnett A. L., (2014). The influence of spelling ability on handwriting production: Children with and without dyslexia. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn Mem. Cogn. 40, 1441–1447. 10.1037/a003578524548322
Tainturier M.-J., Rapp B., (2001). The spelling process, in The Handbook of Cognitive Neuropsychology: What Deficits Reveal about the Human Mind, ed Rapp B., (New York: Psychology Press), 263–289.
Treiman R., (2017). Learning to spell: phonology and beyond. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 34, 83–93. 10.1080/02643294.2017.1337630
Van Galen G. P., (1991). Handwriting: issues for a psychomotor theory. Hum. Mov. Sci. 10, 165–191. 10.1016/0167-9457(91)90003-G
Van Reybroeck M., Hupet M., (2009). Acquisition of number agreement: effects of processing demands. J. Writ. Res. 1, 153–172. 10.17239/jowr2009.01.02.3
Wechsler D., (2005). Échelle D'intelligence de Wechsler Pour Enfants, 4ème éd. [Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children]. Paris: Éditions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.
Wickham R. E., Hardy K. K., Buckman H. L., Lepovic E., (2021). Comparing cross-classified mixed effects and Bayesian structural equations modeling for stimulus sampling designs: a simulation study. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 92:104062. 10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104062
Ziegler J. C., Jacobs A. M., Stone G. O., (1996). Statistical analysis of the bidirectional inconsistency of spelling and sound in French. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 28, 504–515. 10.3758/BF0320053