aging workforce; Belgium; breaks; cognitive fatigue; ergonomics; middle age; organization; Time-on-Task; work; Developmental and Educational Psychology; Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous); Applied Psychology
Abstract :
[en] Maintaining productivity is of primary importance in organizational settings. Nowadays, the pressure for work efficacy is required until advanced age given the increased longevity in western societies. Worryingly, performing a work for a long-lasting duration may induce cognitive fatigue, which can alter job performance or cause work accidents. Regarding laboratory studies, cognitive fatigue, as induced in Time-on-Task designs, has been shown to increase reaction times (RTs). According to the Effort-Recovery Model (ERM), work breaks are able to relieve cognitive fatigue and to maintain performance. However, few studies have investigated age-related effects in such a context. In this study, young, middle-aged, and older people performed a 160-min Stroop task in a “NoBreak” or a “Breaks” condition. To assess changes in RTs with Time-on-Task, the task duration was divided into four 40-min blocks in which the ex-Gaussian τ parameter (i.e., an index of longer RTs) was extracted from individual RT data. Our main results showed that young and middle-aged people increased their τ with Time-on-Task while older people did not. Importantly, participants in the NoBreak condition increased their τ with Time-on-Task while those in the Breaks condition kept this parameter constant, suggesting a beneficial effect of breaks independently of age.
Disciplines :
Neurosciences & behavior
Author, co-author :
Gilsoul, Jessica ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Psychologie > Neuropsychologie
Libertiaux, Vincent ; Université de Liège - ULiège > GIGA > GIGA CRC In vivo Imaging - Neuroimaging, data acquisition and processing
Collette, Fabienne ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Psychologie
Language :
English
Title :
Cognitive fatigue in young, middle-aged, and older: Breaks as a way to recover
Akerstedt, T., & Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. The International Journal of Neuroscience, 52(1–2), 29–37. https://doi.org/10.3109/00207459008994241
Arnau, S., Möckel, T., Rinkenauer, G., & Wascher, E. (2017). The interconnection of mental fatigue and aging: An EEG study. International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 117, 17–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.04.003
Barmack, J. E. (1937). Boredom and other factors in the physiology of mental effort. Archives of Psychology, 31(218), 1–83.
Bennett, A. (2015). Take five? Examining the impact of microbreak duration, activities, and appraisals on human energy and performance. Doctoral dissertation, Virginia Commonwealth University. https://doi.org/10.25772/7DS8-4G13
Bennett, A. A., Bakker, A. B., & Field, J. G. (2018). Recovery from work-related effort: A meta-analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 39(3), 262–275. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2217
Berto, R. (2005). Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25(3), 249–259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.07.001
Bielak, A., Cherbuin, N., Bunce, D., & Anstey, K. (2013). Intraindividual variability is a fundamental phenomenon of aging: Evidence from an 8-year longitudinal study across young, middle, and older adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 50(1), 143–151. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032650
Blain, B., Hollard, G., & Pessiglione, M. (2016). Neural mechanisms underlying the impact of daylong cognitive work on economic decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(25), 6967–6972. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1520527113
Blasche, G., Szabo, B., Wagner-Menghin, M., Ekmekcioglu, C., & Gollner, E. (2018). Comparison of rest-break interventions during a mentally demanding task. Stress and Health, 34(5), 629–638. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.2830
Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: An ERP study. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 107–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011
Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2006). Mental fatigue, motivation and action monitoring. Biological Psychology, 72(2), 123–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.007
Boksem, M. A. S., & Tops, M. (2008). Mental fatigue: Costs and benefits. Brain Research Reviews, 59(1), 125–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.07.001
Bosch, C., Sonnentag, S., & Pinck, A. S. (2018). What makes for a good break? A diary study on recovery experiences during lunch break. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 91(1), 134–157. https://doi.org/10.1111/joop.12195
Brewer, G. A. (2011). Analyzing response time distributions: Methodological and theoretical suggestions for prospective memory researchers. Journal of Psychology, 219(2), 117–124. https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000056
Burbeck, S. L., & Luce, R. D. (1982). Evidence from auditory simple reaction times for both change and level detectors. Perception & Psychophysics, 32(2), 117–133. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03204271
Burke, S. E., Samuel, I. B. H., Zhao, Q., Cagle, J., Cohen, R. A., Kluger, B., & Ding, M. (2018). Task-based cognitive fatigability for older adults and validation of mental fatigability subscore of Pittsburgh Fatigability Scale. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 10(327), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00327
Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., & Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research, 28(2), 193–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4
Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 85–100. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.85
Cansino, S., Estrada-Manilla, C., Trejo-Morales, P., Pasaye-Alcaraz, E. H., Aguilar-Castañeda, E., Salgado-Lujambio, P., & Sosa-Ortiz, A. L. (2015). FMRI subsequent source memory effects in young, middle-aged and old adults. Behavioural Brain Research, 280, 24–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.11.042
Chen, L., Sugi, T., Shirakawa, S., Zou, J., & Nakamura, M. (2010). Integrated design and evaluation system for the effect of rest breaks in sustained mental work based on neuro-physiological signals. International Journal of Control, Automation and Systems, 8, 862–867. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12555-010-0419-x
Christensen, K., Doblhammer, G., Rau, R., & Vaupel, J. W. (2009). Ageing populations: The challenges ahead. Lancet, 374(9696), 1196–1208. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61460-4
Cleveland, J. N., Huebner, L.-A., Anderson, K. J., & Agbeke, D. V. (2019). Chapter 12—Lifespan perspectives on job performance, performance appraisal/management and creative performance. In B. B. Baltes, C. W. Rudolph, & H. Zacher (Eds.), Work across the lifespan (pp. 291–321). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-812756-8.00012-8
Coffeng, J. K., van Sluijs, E. M., Hendriksen, I. J. M., van Mechelen, W., & Boot, C. R. L. (2015). Physical activity and relaxation during and after work are independently associated with the need for recovery. Journal of Physical Activity & Health, 12, 109–115. https://doi.org/10.1123/jpah.2012-0452
Collette, F., & Salmon, E. (2014). Les modifications du fonctionnement exécutif dans le vieillissement normal. Psychologie Française, 59(1), 41–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2013.03.006
Crawford, J. R., Bryan, J., Luszcz, M. A., Obonsawin, M. C., & Stewart, L. (2000). The executive decline hypothesis of cognitive aging: Do executive deficits qualify as differential deficits and do they mediate age-related memory decline? Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 7(1), 9–31. https://doi.org/10.1076/anec.7.1.9.806
Czaja, S. J., Sharit, J., & James, J. B. (Eds.) (2020). Current and emerging trends in aging and work. Springer Nature Switzerland AG. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-24135-3
Dababneh, A. J., Swanson, N., & Shell, R. L. (2001). Impact of added rest breaks on the productivity and well-being of workers. Ergonomics, 44(2), 164–174. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140130121538
Dawson, M. R. W. (1988). Fitting the ex-Gaussian equation to reaction time distributions. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 20(1), 54–57. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202603
de Bloom, J., Kinnunen, U., & Korpela, K. (2015). Recovery processes during and after work: Associations with health, work engagement, and job performance. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 57(7), 732–742. https://doi.org/10.1097/JOM.0000000000000475
de Jong, M., Jolij, J., Pimenta, A., & Lorist, M. M. (2018). Age modulates the effects of mental fatigue on typewriting. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1113. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01113
de Jonge, J., & Peeters, M. C. W. (2019). The vital worker: Towards sustainable performance at work. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(6), 910. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16060910
Deltour, J.-J. (1993). Echelle de vocabulaire Mill Hill de J. C. Raven: Adaptation française et normes comparées du Mill Hill et du Standard Progressive Matrices (PM38). Manuel et Annexes. Application des Techniques Modernes.
Dembe, A. E., Erickson, J. B., Delbos, R. G., & Banks, S. M. (2005). The impact of overtime and long work hours on occupational injuries and illnesses: New evidence from the United States. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 62(9), 588–597. https://doi.org/10.1136/oem.2004.016667
Demerouti, E., Bakker, A. B., Geurts, S. A. E., & Taris, T. W. (2009). Daily recovery from work-related effort during nonwork time. In S. Sonnentag, P. L. Perrewé, & D. C. Ganster (Eds.), Current perspectives on job stress recovery: Research in occupational stress and well-being (pp. 85–123). JAI Press. https://doi.org/10.1108/S1479-3555(2009)0000007006
Dinges, D. F. (1995). An overview of sleepiness and accidents. Journal of Sleep Research, 4(2), 2–14. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.1995.tb00220.x
Duncan, M. J., Dobell, A. P., Caygill, C. L., Eyre, E., & Tallis, J. (2019). The effect of acute caffeine ingestion on upper body anaerobic exercise and cognitive performance. European Journal of Sport Science, 19(1), 103–111. https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2018.1508505
Eckert, M. A., Keren, N. I., Roberts, D. R., Calhoun, V. D., & Harris, K. C. (2010). Age-related changes in processing speed: Unique contributions of cerebellar and prefrontal cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.010.2010
Eder, P., & Sawyer, J. E. (2007). A meta-analytic examination of employee creativity. In 22nd Annual Conference. Society of Industrial and Organizational Psychology (SIOP), New York, NY, April.
Engelmann, C., Schneider, M., Kirschbaum, C., Grote, G., Dingemann, J., Schoof, S., & Ure, B. M. (2011). Effects of intraoperative breaks on mental and somatic operator fatigue: A randomized clinical trial. Surgical Endoscopy, 25(4), 1245–1250. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00464-010-1350-1
Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., & Hohnsbein, J. (2002). Inhibition-related ERP components: Variation with modality, age, and Time-on-Task. Journal of Psychophysiology, 16(3), 167–175. https://doi.org/10.1027//0269-8803.16.3.167
Falkenstein, M., Yordanova, J., & Kolev, V. (2006). Effects of aging on slowing of motor-response generation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 59(1), 22–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.08.004
Farnsworth, D. (1947). The Farnsworth dichotomous test for color blindness, Panel D-15: Manual. The Psychological Corp.
Finkbeiner, K. M., Russell, P. N., & Helton, W. S. (2016). Rest improves performance, nature improves happiness: Assessment of break periods on the abbreviated vigilance task. Consciousness and Cognition, 42, 277–285. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.04.005
Fritz, C., Ellis, A. M., Demsky, C. A., Lin, B. C., & Guros, F. (2013). Embracing work breaks: Recovering from work stress. Organizational Dynamics, 42(4), 274–280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.orgdyn.2013.07.005
Fritz, C., & Sonnentag, S. (2005). Recovery, health, and job performance: Effects of weekend experiences. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 10, 187–199. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.10.3.187
Gilsoul, J., Libertiaux, V., & Collette, F. (2021). Cognitive fatigue in young, middle-aged, and older: A response time distribution approach. Under Review.
Grandjean, C., McMullen, P., Miller, K., Howie, W., Ryan, K., Myers, A., & Dutton, R. (2006). Severe occupational injuries among older workers: Demographic factors, time of injury, place and mechanism of injury, length of stay, and cost data. Nursing and Health Sciences, 8(2), 103–107. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1442-2018.2006.00260.x
Grillon, C., Quispe-Escudero, D., Mathur, A., & Ernst, M. (2015). Mental fatigue impairs emotion regulation. Emotion, 15(3), 383–389. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000058
Hancock, P. A., & Desmond, P. A. (2000). Stress, workload, and fatigue. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. https://doi.org/10.1201/b12791
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 193–225). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60041-9
He, X., Qin, W., Liu, Y., Zhang, X., Duan, Y., Song, J., Li, K., Jiang, T., & Yu, C. (2013). Age-related decrease in functional connectivity of the right fronto-insular cortex with the central executive and default-mode networks in adults from young to middle age. Neuroscience Letters, 544, 74–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2013.03.044
Heathcote, A., Popiel, S. J., & Mewhort, D. J. (1991). Analysis of response time distributions: An example using the Stroop task. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 340–347. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.340
Helton, W. S., & Russell, P. N. (2015). Rest is best: The role of rest and task interruptions on vigilance. Cognition, 134, 165–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.001
Herzog, T. R., Black, A. M., Fountaine, K. A., & Knotts, D. J. (1997). Reflection and attentional recovery as distinctive benefits of restorative environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 17(2), 165–170. https://doi.org/10.1006/jevp.1997.0051
Hockey, G. R. J. (2011). A motivational control theory of cognitive fatigue. In P. L. Ackerman (Ed.), Cognitive fatigue: Multidisciplinary perspectives on current research and future applications (pp. 167–188). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/12343-008
Hockey, G. R. J. (2013). The psychology of fatigue: Work, effort and control. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139015394
Hohle, R. H. (1965). Inferred components of reaction times as functions of foreperiod duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 382–386. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0021740
Hopstaken, J. F., van der Linden, D., Bakker, A. B., & Kompier, M. A. J. (2015a). A multifaceted investigation of the link between mental fatigue and task disengagement. Psychophysiology, 52(3), 305–315. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12339
Hopstaken, J. F., van der Linden, D., Bakker, A. B., & Kompier, M. A. J. (2015b). The window of my eyes: Task disengagement and mental fatigue covary with pupil dynamics. Biological Psychology, 110, 100–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.06.013
Hopstaken, J. F., van der Linden, D., Bakker, A. B., Kompier, M. A. J., & Leung, Y. K. (2016). Shifts in attention during mental fatigue: Evidence from subjective, behavioral, physiological, and eye-tracking data. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(6), 878–889. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000189
Horne, J. A., & Ostberg, O. (1976). A self assessment questionnaire to determine morningness eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology, 4, 97–110.
Hu, Z., Yi, C., Hao, J., Qiao, X., & Guo, X. (2018). Comparative study on the effects of lighting on cognitive ergonomics in single and multi-working modes. NeuroQuantology, 16(5), 341–349. https://doi.org/10.14704/nq.2018.16.5.1290
Hunter, E. M., & Wu, C. (2016). Give me a better break: Choosing workday break activities to maximize resource recovery. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101(2), 302–311. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000045
Johns, M. W. (1991). A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep, 14(6), 540–545. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/14.6.540
Kaida, K., Takahashi, M., Akerstedt, T., Nakata, A., Otsuka, Y., Haratani, T., & Fukasawa, K. (2006). Validation of the Karolinska sleepiness scale against performance and EEG variables. Clinical Neurophysiology: Official Journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology, 117(7), 1574–1581. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2006.03.011
Kaplan, R., & Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: A psychological perspective. Cambridge University Press.
Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15(3), 169–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2
Kato, Y., Endo, H., & Kizuka, T. (2009). Mental fatigue and impaired response processes: Event-related brain potentials in a Go/NoGo task. International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 72(2), 204–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.12.008
Kemmlert, K., & Lundholm, L. (2001). Slips, trips and falls in different work groups—With reference to age and from a preventive perspective. Applied Ergonomics, 32(2), 149–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0003-6870(00)00051-x
Kim, S., Park, Y., & Niu, Q. (2017). Micro-break activities at work to recover from daily work demands. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 38(1), 28–44. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2109
Kira, M., & Lifvergren, S. (2014). Sowing seeds for sustainability in work systems. In I. Ehnert, W. Harry, & K. L. Zink (Eds.), Sustainability and human resource management (pp. 56–82). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-37524-8_3
Klaassen, E. B., Evers, E. A. T., de Groot, R. H. M., Backes, W. H., Veltman, D. J., & Jolles, J. (2014). Working memory in middle-aged males: Age-related brain activation changes and cognitive fatigue effects. Biological Psychology, 96, 134–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.008
Klaassen, E. B., Plukaard, S., Evers, E. A. T., de Groot, R. H. M., Backes, W. H., Veltman, D. J., & Jolles, J. (2016). Young and middle-aged schoolteachers differ in the neural correlates of memory encoding and cognitive fatigue: A functional MRI study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10(148), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00148
Kühnel, J., Zacher, H., de Bloom, J., & Bledow, R. (2017). Take a break! Benefits of sleep and short breaks for daily work engagement. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 26(4), 481–491. https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2016.1269750
Lacouture, Y., & Cousineau, D. (2008). How to use MATLAB to fit the ex-Gaussian and other probability functions to response times. Tutorial in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4(1), 35–45. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.04.1.p035
Lang, J. W. B., Kersting, M., Hülsheger, U. R., & Lang, J. (2010). General mental ability, narrower cognitive abilities, and job performance: The perspective of the nested-factors model of cognitive abilities. Personnel Psychology, 63(3), 595–640. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2010.01182.x
Lee, K. E., Williams, K. J. H., Sargent, L. D., Williams, N. S. G., & Johnson, K. A. (2015). 40-second green roof views sustain attention: The role of micro-breaks in attention restoration. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 42, 182–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.04.003
Lenth, R. (2018). lsmeans: Least-squares means (version 2.30-0) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lsmeans
Lim, J., & Kwok, K. (2016). The effects of varying break length on attention and time on task. Human Factors, 58(3), 472–481. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720815617395
Lim, J., Teng, J., Wong, K. F., & Chee, M. W. L. (2016). Modulating rest-break length induces differential recruitment of automatic and controlled attentional processes upon task reengagement. NeuroImage, 134, 64–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.077
Lorist, M. M. (2008). Impact of top-down control during mental fatigue. Brain Research, 1232, 113–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.053
Lorist, M. M., Bezdan, E., ten Caat, M., Span, M. M., Roerdink, J. B. T. M., & Maurits, N. M. (2009). The influence of mental fatigue and motivation on neural network dynamics; an EEG coherence study. Brain Research, 1270, 95–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009.03.015
Lorist, M. M., Boksem, M. A. S., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Impaired cognitive control and reduced cingulate activity during mental fatigue. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 199–205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.018
Lorist, M. M., Klein, M., Nieuwenhuis, S., Jong, R., Mulder, G., & Meijman, T. (2000). Mental fatigue and task control: Planning and preparation. Psychophysiology, 37, 614–625. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3750614
Luce, R. D. (1986). Response times. Oxford University Press.
MacLeod, C. M., & MacDonald, P. A. (2000). Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: Uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(10), 383–391. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01530-8
Maltese, F., Adda, M., Bablon, A., Hraeich, S., Guervilly, C., Lehingue, S., Wiramus, S., Leone, M., Martin, C., Vialet, R., Thirion, X., Roch, A., Forel, J.-M., & Papazian, L. (2016). Night shift decreases cognitive performance of ICU physicians. Intensive Care Medicine, 42(3), 393–400. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00134-015-4115-4
Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W. B., & Leiter, M. P. (2001). Job burnout. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 397–422. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.397
Mathiassen, S. E., Hallman, D. M., Lyskov, E., & Hygge, S. (2014). Can cognitive activities during breaks in repetitive manual work accelerate recovery from fatigue? A controlled experiment. PLoS ONE, 9(11), e112090. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112090
Mattis, S. (1976). Dementia rating scale. In R. Bellack & B. Keraso (Eds.), Geriatric psychiatry X (pp. 77–121). Grune and Stratton.
May, J. F., & Baldwin, C. L. (2009). Driver fatigue: The importance of identifying causal factors of fatigue when considering detection and countermeasure technologies. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 12(3), 218–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2008.11.005
McCormick, F., Kadzielski, J., Landrigan, C. P., Evans, B., Herndon, J. H., & Rubash, H. E. (2012). Surgeon fatigue: A prospective analysis of the incidence, risk, and intervals of predicted fatigue-related impairment in residents. Archives of Surgery, 147(5), 430–435. https://doi.org/10.1001/archsurg.2012.84
Meijman, T. F., & Mulder, G. (1998). Psychological aspects of workload. In P. J. D. Drenth, H. Thierry, & C. J. de Wolff (Eds.), Handbook of work and organizational: Work psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 2) (pp. 5–33). Psychology Press/Erlbaum (UK) Taylor & Francis.
Mijović, P., Ković, V., Mačužić, I., Todorović, P., Jeremić, B., Milovanović, M., & Gligorijević, I. (2015). Do micro-breaks increase the attention level of an assembly worker? An ERP study. Procedia Manufacturing, 3, 5074–5080. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.promfg.2015.07.521
Monsch, A. U., Bondi, M. W., Salmon, D. P., Butters, N., Thal, L. J., Hansen, L. A., Wiederholt, W. C., Cahn, D. A., & Klauber, M. R. (1995). Clinical validity of the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale in detecting dementia of the Alzheimer type. A double cross-validation and application to a community-dwelling sample. Archives of Neurology, 52(9), 899–904. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1995.00540330081018
Mullette-Gillman, O. A., Leong, R. L. F., & Kurnianingsih, Y. A. (2015). Cognitive fatigue destabilizes economic decision making preferences and strategies. PLoS ONE, 10(7), e0132022. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132022
Myers, C. S. (1937). Conceptions of mental fatigue. The American Journal of Psychology, 50, 296–306. https://doi.org/10.2307/1416638
Naegele, G., & Walker, A. (2006). A guide to good practice in age management. European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions. Retrieved from. http://www.ageingatwork.eu/resources/a-guide-to-good-practice-in-age-management.pdf
Nelder, J., & Mead, R. (1965). A simplex method for function minimization. Computer Journal, 7(4), 308–313. https://doi.org/10.1093/comjnl/7.4.308
Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2008). The relationship of age to ten dimensions of job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(2), 392–423. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.2.392
Norouzi, A. (2019). Employee one-minute mindful breathing microbreaks for present-moment attention, recovery, and well-being [Doctoral Dissertation, Grand Canyon University]. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing. 27735085
OECD. (2011). Pensions at a glance. Retirement-income systems in OECD and G20 countries. OECD. https://doi.org/10.1787/19991363
Okun, M. A., Barr, A., & Herzog, A. R. (1998). Motivation to volunteer by older adults: A test of competing measurement models. Psychology and Aging, 13(4), 608–621. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.13.4.608
Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 173–196. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656
Park, H., Kennedy, K. M., Rodrigue, K. M., Hebrank, A., & Park, D. C. (2013). An fMRI study of episodic encoding across the lifespan: Changes in subsequent memory effects are evident by middle-age. Neuropsychologia, 51(3), 448–456. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.11.025
Philip, P., Taillard, J., Quera-Salva, M. A., Bioulac, B., & Akerstedt, T. (1999). Simple reaction time, duration of driving and sleep deprivation in young versus old automobile drivers. Journal of Sleep Research, 8(1), 9–14. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2869.1999.00127.x
Phillipson, C., Vickerstaff, S., & Lain, D. (2016). Achieving fuller working lives: Labour market and policy issues in the United Kingdom. Australian Journal of Social Issues, 51(2), 187–203. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1839-4655.2016.tb00373.x
Phipps-Nelson, J., Redman, J. R., & Rajaratnam, S. M. W. (2010). Temporal profile of prolonged, night-time driving performance: Breaks from driving temporarily reduce time-on-task fatigue but not sleepiness. Journal of Sleep Research, 20(3), 404–415. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00900.x
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., & R-Core Team. (2020). nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models (version 3.1–145) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme
Powell, D., Spencer, M., Holland, D., & Petrie, K. (2008). Fatigue in two-pilot operations: Implications for flight and duty time limitations. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 79, 1047–1050. https://doi.org/10.3357/ASEM.2362.2008
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385–401. https://doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306
Ratcliff, R., & Murdock, B. B. (1976). Retrieval processes in recognition memory. Psychological Review, 83(3), 190–214. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.83.3.190
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 177–182. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00570.x
Rey-Mermet, A., & Gade, M. (2018). Inhibition in aging: What is preserved? What declines? A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(5), 1695–1716. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1384-7
Richter, S., Marsalek, K., Glatz, C., & Gundel, A. (2005). Task-dependent differences in subjective fatigue scores. Journal of Sleep Research, 14(4), 393–400. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2005.00473.x
Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). ‘Oops!’: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), 747–758. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00015-8
Roggeveen, A. B., Prime, D. J., & Ward, L. M. (2007). Lateralized readiness potentials reveal motor slowing in the aging brain. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 62(2), 78–84. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/62.2.P78
Rosa, R. R., & Bonnet, M. H. (1993). Performance and alertness on 8 h and 12 h rotating shifts at a natural gas utility. Ergonomics, 36(10), 1177–1193. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140139308967987
Ross, H. A., Russell, P. N., & Helton, W. S. (2014). Effects of breaks and goal switches on the vigilance decrement. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 1729–1737. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-3865-5
Salthouse, T. A. (1979). Adult age and the speed-accuracy trade-off. Ergonomics, 22, 811–821. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140137908924659
Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., & Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(4), 566–594. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.132.4.566
Santrock, J. W. (2015). Life-span development (15th ed.). McGraw-Hill.
Schmiedek, F., Oberauer, K., Wilhelm, O., Süß, H.-M., & Wittmann, W. W. (2007). Individual differences in components of reaction time distributions and their relations to working memory and intelligence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(3), 414–429. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.3.414
Schulte, P. A., Grosch, J., Scholl, J. C., & Tamers, S. L. (2018). Framework for considering productive aging and work. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 60(5), 440–448. https://doi.org/10.1097/jom.0000000000001295
Schwatka, N. V., Butler, L. M., & Rosecrance, J. R. (2012). An aging workforce and injury in the construction industry. Epidemiologic Reviews, 34, 156–167. https://doi.org/10.1093/epirev/mxr020
Shen, K.-Q., Li, X.-P., Ong, C.-J., Shao, S.-Y., & Wilder-Smith, E. P. V. (2008). EEG-based mental fatigue measurement using multi-class support vector machines with confidence estimate. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119(7), 1524–1533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2008.03.012
Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., & Mojza, E. J. (2008). “Did you have a nice evening?” A day-level study on recovery experiences, sleep, and affect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 674–684. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.3.674
Sonnentag, S., & Fritz, C. (2007). The Recovery Experience Questionnaire: Development and validation of a measure for assessing recuperation and unwinding from work. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12, 204–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.12.3.204
Sonnentag, S., Venz, L., & Casper, A. (2017). Advances in recovery research: What have we learned? What should be done next? Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 22(3), 365–380. https://doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000079
Steenstra, I., Cullen, K., Irvin, E., Van Eerd, D., Alavinia, M., Beaton, D., Geary, J., Gignac, M., Gross, D., Mahood, Q., & Macdonald, S. (2017). A systematic review of interventions to promote work participation in older workers. Journal of Safety Research, 60, 93–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsr.2016.12.004
Streb, C. K., Voelpel, S. C., & Leibold, M. (2008). Managing the aging workforce: Status quo and implications for the advancement of theory and practice. European Management Journal, 26(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.emj.2007.08.004
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651
Strozyk, J. V., & Jentzsch, I. (2012). Weaker error signals do not reduce the effectiveness of post-error adjustments: Comparing error processing in young and middle-aged adults. Brain Research, 1460, 41–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.04.028
Tanaka, M., & Watanabe, Y. (2010). A new hypothesis of chronic fatigue syndrome: Co-conditioning theory. Medical Hypotheses, 75(2), 244–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2010.02.032
Terentjeviene, A., Maciuleviciene, E., Vadopalas, K., Mickeviciene, D., Karanauskiene, D., Valanciene, D., Solianik, R., Emeljanovas, A., Kamandulis, S., & Skurvydas, A. (2018). Prefrontal cortex activity predicts mental fatigue in young and elderly men during a 2 h “Go/NoGo” task. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12(620), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00620
Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section a, 37(4), 571–590. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748508400920
Trougakos, J. P., Beal, D. J., Green, S. G., & Weiss, H. M. (2008). Making the break count: An episodic examination of recovery activities, emotional experiences, and positive affective displays. Academy of Management Journal, 51, 131–146. https://doi.org/10.5465/AMJ.2008.30764063
Trougakos, J. P., & Hideg, I. (2009). Momentary work recovery: The role of within-day work breaks. In S. Sonnentag, P. L. Perrewé, & D. C. Ganster (Eds.), Current perspectives on job-stress recovery (pp. 37–84). Emerald Group. https://doi.org/10.1108/S1479-3555(2009)0000007005
Tucker, P., Folkard, S., & Macdonald, I. (2003). Rest breaks and accident risk. Lancet, 361(9358), 680. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(03)12566-4
Unsworth, N., Redick, T. S., Lakey, C. E., & Young, D. L. (2010). Lapses in sustained attention and their relation to executive control and fluid abilities: An individual differences investigation. Intelligence, 38(1), 111–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2009.08.002
van der Linden, D., Frese, M., & Meijman, T. F. (2003). Mental fatigue and the control of cognitive processes: Effects on perseveration and planning. Acta Psychologica, 113(1), 45–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0001-6918(02)00150-6
Varianou-Mikellidou, C., Boustras, G., Dimopoulos, C., Wybo, J.-L., Guldenmund, F. W., Nicolaidou, O., & Anyfantis, I. (2019). Occupational health and safety management in the context of an ageing workforce. Safety Science, 116, 231–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2019.03.009
Vilagut, G., Forero, C. G., Barbaglia, G., & Alonso, J. (2016). Screening for depression in the general population with the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D): A systematic review with meta-analysis. PLoS ONE, 11(5), e0155431. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0155431
Wang, C., Ding, M., & Kluger, B. M. (2014). Change in intraindividual variability over time as a key metric for defining performance-based cognitive fatigability. Brain and Cognition, 85, 251–258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.004
Wascher, E., Heppner, H., Kobald, S. O., Arnau, S., Getzmann, S., & Möckel, T. (2016). Age-sensitive effects of enduring work with alternating cognitive and physical load. A study applying mobile EEG in a real life working scenario. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9(711), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00711
Wendsche, J., Lohmann-Haislah, A., & Wegge, J. (2016). The impact of supplementary short rest breaks on task performance—A meta-analysis. Sozialpolitik.ch, 2, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.18753/2297-8224-75
West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120(2), 272–292. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.272
West, R. L. (2000). In defense of the frontal lobe hypothesis of cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(6), 727–729. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700666109
Winston, N., & Barnes, J. (2007). Anticipation of retirement among baby boomers. Journal of Women & Aging, 19, 137–159. https://doi.org/10.1300/J074v19n03_10
Wolkorte, R., Kamphuis, J., & Zijdewind, I. (2014). Increased reaction times and reduced response preparation already starts at middle age. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6(79), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00079
Wood, S., Michaelides, G., & Totterdell, P. (2013). The impact of fluctuating workloads on well-being and the mediating role of work-nonwork interference in this relationship. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 18(1), 106–119. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031067
Yordanova, J., Kolev, V., Hohnsbein, J., & Falkenstein, M. (2004). Sensorimotor slowing with ageing is mediated by a functional dysregulation of motor-generation processes: Evidence from high-resolution event-related potentials. Brain, 127(2), 351–362. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh042
Zacher, H. (2015). Successful aging at work. Work, Aging and Retirement, 1(1), 4–25. https://doi.org/10.1093/workar/wau006
Zacher, H., Brailsford, H. A., & Parker, S. L. (2014). Micro-breaks matter: A diary study on the effects of energy management strategies on occupational well-being. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 85(3), 287–297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2014.08.005