convergent thinking – education; creativity development; divergent thinking; Montessori; Education; Developmental and Educational Psychology; Visual Arts and Performing Arts
Résumé :
[en] Creative thinking is critical to overcome many daily life situations. As such, there has been a growing interest on how creative thinking develops during childhood. However, little is known about the underlying mechanisms driving its development. Indeed, almost all research has focused on divergent thinking, leaving aside convergent thinking, and did not thoroughly investigate how internal and/or external factors influence their development. Here, 222 children aged from 4 to 12 years old attending either a Montessori or a traditional school performed drawing-based convergent and divergent standardized tasks. In addition, a subset of 41 children were tested using similar tasks for a second session 3 years apart. The results revealed dynamic developmental stages of convergent and divergent thinking. More specifically, a loss of divergent thinking was counterbalanced by a gain of convergent thinking, especially during the fourth-grade slump (8–10 years old). Although Montessori-schooled children showed overall higher creative abilities than traditionally schooled children, no differences were observed in the developmental trajectories of convergent and divergent thinking between the two pedagogies. This suggests that progress and decrease in creative thinking may be mostly due to internal factors such as brain maturation factors than external factors such as peer pressure.
Disciplines :
Psychologie cognitive & théorique
Auteur, co-auteur :
Eon Duval, Philippe; Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Radiology, Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and University of Lausanne (UNIL), Lausanne, Switzerland
Frick, Aurélien ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département des sciences cliniques
Denervaud, Solange ; Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Radiology, Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV) and University of Lausanne (UNIL), Lausanne, Switzerland
Langue du document :
Anglais
Titre :
Divergent and Convergent Thinking across the Schoolyears: A Dynamic Perspective on Creativity Development
SNF - Schweizerische Nationalfonds zur Förderung der wissenschaftlichen Forschung
Subventionnement (détails) :
We are grateful to the children and parents who partook in this study. We thank Morgane Budry, Diane Gervaise, Julien Fedrizzi, Claire Batilotti, Marion Décaillet, Coline Grasset, and Amie Chiron for helping with data collection. Imaging was supported in part by the Centre d'Imagerie Biomédicale (CIBM) of the Université de Lausanne (UNIL), Université de Genève (UNIGE), Hôpitaux Universitaires de Genève (HUG), Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), and the Leenaards and Jeantet Foundations. This research was financially supported by the National Center of Competence in Research in (NCCR) Affective sciences; by the Swiss National Science Foundation (n° 51NF40‐104897); by the Boninchi Foundation; by the Fondation Eden in Geneva; by The Société Académique Vaudoise; and by The Prepared Adult Initiative.
scite shows how a scientific paper has been cited by providing the context of the citation, a classification describing whether it supports, mentions, or contrasts the cited claim, and a label indicating in which section the citation was made.
Bibliographie
Amabile, T.M., Conti, R., Coon, H., Lazenby, J., & Herron, M. (1996). Assessing the work environment for creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 39(5), 1154–1184; doi: 10.2307/256995.
Barbot, B. (2018). The dynamics of creative ideation: Introducing a new assessment paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2529; doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02529.
Barbot, B., Besançon, M., & Lubart, T.I. (2011). Assessing creativity in the classroom. The Open Education Journal, 4(1), 58–66; doi: 10.2174/1874920801104010058.
Barbot, B., Lubart, T.I., & Besançon, M. (2016). “Peaks, Slumps, and Bumps”: Individual differences in the development of creativity in children and adolescents. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2016(151), 33–45; doi: 10.1002/CAD.20152.
Beaty, R., & Johnson, D. (2020). Automating creativity assessment with SemDis: An open platform for computing semantic distance. Behavior Research Methods, 53, 757–780; doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01453-w.
Beaty, R.E., Nusbaum, E.C., & Silvia, P.J. (2014). Does insight problem solving predict real-world creativity? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(3), 287–292; doi: 10.1037/A0035727.
Besançon, M., & Lubart, T. (2007). Differences in the development of creative competencies in children schooled in diverse learning environments. Learning and Individual Differences, 18(4), 381–389; doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2007.11.009.
Camarda, A., Bouhours, L., Osmont, A., Le Masson, P., Weil, B., Borst, G., & Cassotti, M. (2021). Opposite effect of social evaluation on creative idea generation in early and middle adolescents. Creativity Research Journal, 33(4), 399–410; doi: 10.1080/10400419.2021.1902174.
Castillo-Vergara, M., Barrios Galleguillos, N., Jofré Cuello, L., Alvarez-Marin, A., & Acuña-Opazo, C. (2018). Does socioeconomic status influence student creativity? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 29, 142–152; doi: 10.1016/J.TSC.2018.07.005.
Charles, R.E., & Runco, M.A. (2010). Developmental trends in the evaluative and divergent thinking of children. Creativity Research Journal, 13(3–4), 417–437; doi: 10.1207/S15326934CRJ1334_19.
Claxton, A.F., Pannells, T.C., & Rhoads, P.A. (2005). Developmental trends in the creativity of school-age children. Creativity Research Journal, 17(4), 327–335; doi: 10.1207/S15326934CRJ1704_4.
Denervaud, S., Christensen, A.P., Kenett, Y.N., & Beaty, R.E. (2021). Education shapes the structure of semantic memory and impacts creative thinking. Npj Science of Learning, 6, 35; doi: 10.1038/s41539-021-00113-8.
Dineen, R., & Niu, W. (2008). The effectiveness of western creative teaching methods in China: An action research project. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2(1), 42–52; doi: 10.1037/1931-3896.2.1.42.
Fan, F., Liao, X., Lei, T., Zhao, T., Xia, M., Men, W., … He, Y. (2021). Development of the default-mode network during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal resting-state fMRI study. NeuroImage, 226, 117581; doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2020.117581.
Fischer, K.W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87(6), 477–531; doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.87.6.477.
Genoud, P.A. (2011). Indice de position socioéconomique: Un calcul simplifié. Fribourg: Université de Fribourg. www.unifr.ch/cerf/ipse.
Gitten, J.C., Winer, J.L., Festa, E.K., & Heindel, W.C. (2006). Conditional associative learning of spatial and object information in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 12(1), 39–56; doi: 10.1080/09297040500205579.
Gralewski, J., Lebuda, I., Gajda, A., Jankowska, D.M., & Wiśniewska, E. (2016). Slumps and jumps: Another look at developmental changes in creative abilities. Creativity. Theories – Research – Applications, 3(1), 152–177; doi: 10.1515/CTRA-2016-0011.
Guignard, J.H., & Lubart, T.I. (2016). A comparative study of convergent and divergent thinking in intellectually gifted children. Gifted and Talented International, 22(1), 9–15; doi: 10.1080/15332276.2007.11673481.
Guilford, J.P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1951-04354-001.
Hammershøj, L.G. (2021). Creativity in children as play and humour: Indicators of affective processes of creativity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 39, 100784; doi: 10.1016/J.TSC.2020.100784.
Hayek, A.S., Toma, C., Guidotti, S., Oberlé, D., & Butera, F. (2017). Grades degrade group coordination: Deteriorated interactions and performance in a cooperative motor task. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 32(1), 97–112; doi: 10.1007/s10212-016-0286-9.
Jaarsveld, S., Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2012). Creative reasoning across developmental levels: Convergence and divergence in problem creation. Intelligence, 40(2), 172–188; doi: 10.1016/J.INTELL.2012.01.002.
Kleibeuker, S.W., De Dreu, C.K.W., & Crone, E.A. (2013). The development of creative cognition across adolescence: Distinct trajectories for insight and divergent thinking. Developmental Science, 16(1), 2–12; doi: 10.1111/J.1467-7687.2012.01176.X.
Lau, S., & Cheung, P.C. (2010). Developmental trends of creativity: What twists of turn do boys and girls take at different grades? Creativity Research Journal, 22(3), 329–336; doi: 10.1080/10400419.2010.503543.
Lillard, A., & Else-Quest, N. (2006). Evaluating Montessori education. Science, 313(5795), 1893–1894; doi: 10.1126/science.1132362.
Lillard, A., Heise, M.J., Richey, E.M., Tong, X., Hart, A., & Bray, P.M. (2017). Montessori preschool elevates and equalizes child outcomes: A longitudinal study. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1783; doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01783.
Little T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling (Methodology in the social sciences). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Lubart, T., Besançon, M., & Barbot, B. (2011). EPoC: Evaluation du potentiel créatif des enfants. France: Hogrefe.
Marshall, C. (2017). Montessori education: A review of the evidence base. Science of Learning, 2(1), 1–9; doi: 10.1038/s41539-017-0012-7.
Miller, B.C., & Gerard, D. (1979). Family influences on the development of creativity in children: An integrative review. The Family Coordinator, 28(3), 295–312; doi: 10.2307/581942.
Montessori, M. (1936). The Secret Of Childhood. Carter, barbara Barclay Ed. https://archive.org/details/in.ernet.dli.2015.443380/page/n9/mode/2up
Mouchiroud, C., & Lubart, T. (2016). Social creativity: A cross-sectional study of 6- to 11-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(1), 60–69; doi: 10.1177/016502540202600111.
Munakata, Y., Snyder, H.R., & Chatham, C.H. (2012). Developing cognitive control: Three key transitions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 71; doi: 10.1177/0963721412436807.
Paulus, P.B., & Dzindolet, M. (2008). Social influence, creativity and innovation. Social Influence, 3(4), 228–247; doi: 10.1080/15534510802341082.
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York, NY: W W Norton & Co.; doi: 10.1037/11494-000.
Piaget, J. (1954). The development of object concept. In J. Piaget & M. Cook (Trans.), The Construction of Reality in the Child (pp. 3–96). New York, NY: Basic Books; doi: 10.1037/11168-001.
Piaw, C.Y. (2014). Effects of gender and thinking style on student's creative thinking ability. Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sciences, 116, 5135–5139; doi: 10.1016/J.SBSPRO.2014.01.1087.
Plucker, J.A., & Renzulli, J.S. (1999). Psychometric approaches to the study of human creativity. Handbook of Creativity, 35–61. doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511807916.005.
R Studio Team. (2020). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Boston, MA. http://www.rstudio.com/.
Raina, M.K., Kumar, G., & Raina, V.K. (1980). A cross-cultural study of parental perception of the ideal child. Creative Child & Adult Quarterly, 5(4), 234–241.
Raven, B.H., Schwarzwald, J., & Koslowsky, M. (1998). Conceptualizing and measuring a power/interaction model of interpersonal influence1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28(4), 307–332. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01708.x.
Rawlings, B., & Legare, C.H. (2021). Toddlers, tools, and tech: The cognitive ontogenesis of innovation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(1), 81–92; doi: 10.1016/J.TICS.2020.10.006.
Runco, M.A., & Acar, S. (2019). Divergent thinking. In J.C. Kaufman & R.J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity (Vol. 224–254). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; doi: 10.1017/9781316979839.013.
Saggar, M., Xie, H., Beaty, R.E., Stankov, A.D., Schreier, M., & Reiss, A.L. (2019). Creativity slumps and bumps: Examining the neurobehavioral basis of creativity development during middle childhood. NeuroImage, 196, 94–101; doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2019.03.080.
Said-Metwaly, S., Fernández-Castilla, B., Kyndt, E., Van den Noortgate, W., & Barbot, B. (2021). Does the fourth-grade slump in creativity actually exist? A meta-analysis of the development of divergent thinking in school-age children and adolescents. Educational Psychology Review, 33(1), 275–298; doi: 10.1007/S10648-020-09547-9/TABLES/2.
Sak, U., & Maker, C.J. (2006). Developmental variation in children's creative mathematical thinking as a function of schooling, age, and knowledge. Creativity Research Journal, 18(3), 279–291; doi: 10.1207/S15326934CRJ1803_5.
Sauzéon, H., Lestage, P., Raboutet, C., N'Kaoua, B., & Claverie, B. (2004). Verbal fluency output in children aged 7–16 as a function of the production criterion: Qualitative analysis of clustering, switching processes, and semantic network exploitation. Brain and Language, 89(1), 192–202; doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00367-5.
Shao, Y., Zhang, C., Zhou, J., Gu, T., & Yuan, Y. (2019). How does culture shape creativity? A mini-review. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(MAY), 1219; doi: 10.3389/FPSYG.2019.01219/BIBTEX.
Snyder, H.R., & Munakata, Y. (2010). Becoming self-directed: Abstract representations support endogenous Flexibility in children. Cognition, 116(2), 155–167; doi: 10.1016/J.COGNITION.2010.04.007.
Snyder, H.R., & Munakata, Y. (2013). So many options, so little control: Abstract representations can reduce selection demands to increase children's self-directed flexibility. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(3), 659–673; doi: 10.1016/J.JECP.2013.07.010.
Sternberg, R.J., & Lubart, T.I. (1996). Investing in creativity. American Psychologist, 51(7), 677–688; doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.51.7.677.
Stevenson, C.E., Kleibeuker, S.W., de Dreu, C.K.W., & Crone, E.A. (2014). Training creative cognition: Adolescence as a flexible period for improving creativity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 827; doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2014.00827/BIBTEX.
Timmel, J.L. (2001). Creativity and acculturation: Psychological and cultural effects on the divergent thinking of cuban preadolescent immigrants entering the united states. https://www.proquest.com/dissertations-theses/creativity-acculturation-psychological-cultural/docview/304690410/se-2?accountid=12006
Torrance, E.P. (1968). A longitudinal examination of the fourth grade slump in creativity. Gifted Child Quarterly, 12(4), 195–199; doi: 10.1177/001698626801200401.
Publications similaires
Sorry the service is unavailable at the moment. Please try again later.
L’Université de Liège souhaite utiliser des cookies ou traceurs pour stocker et accéder à des données à caractère personnel vous concernant pour effectuer des mesures d’audience et permettre des fonctionnalités liées aux réseaux sociaux. Certains cookies sont nécessaires au fonctionnement du site.
Vous pouvez autoriser ou refuser tout ou partie de ces traitements de données qui sont basés sur votre consentement, à l\'exception des cookies et/ou traceurs nécessaires au fonctionnement de ce site. Vous pouvez modifier vos choix à tout moment.En savoir plus
Sauvegarder et fermer
Accepter tout
Refuser tout
Afficher les détailsMasquer les détails
Déclaration relative aux cookies
À propos des cookies
Strictement nécessaires
Performance
Les cookies strictement nécessaires habilitent des fonctionnalités de base du site Web telles que la connexion des utilisateurs et la gestion des comptes. Le site Web ne peut pas être utilisé correctement sans les cookies strictement nécessaires.
Cookie de session de plate-forme à usage général, utilisé par les sites écrits en JSP. Habituellement utilisé pour maintenir une session utilisateur anonyme par le serveur.
Ce cookie est utilisé par le service Cookie-Script.com pour mémoriser les préférences de consentement des visiteurs en matière de cookies. Il est nécessaire pour que la bannière de cookies Cookie-Script.com fonctionne correctement.
Les cookies de performance sont utilisés pour voir comment les visiteurs utilisent le site Web, par exemple les cookies d\'analyse. Ces cookies ne peuvent pas être utilisés pour identifier directement un visiteur spécifique.
Ce nom de cookie est associé à la plateforme d'analyse Web open source Matomo. Il est utilisé pour aider les propriétaires de sites Web à suivre le comportement des visiteurs et à mesurer les performances du site. Il s'agit d'un cookie de type modèle, où le préfixe _pk_id est suivi d'une courte série de chiffres et de lettres, qui est censé être un code de référence pour le domaine définissant le cookie.
Ce nom de cookie est associé à la plateforme d'analyse Web open source Matomo. Il est utilisé pour aider les propriétaires de sites Web à suivre le comportement des visiteurs et à mesurer les performances du site. Il s'agit d'un cookie de type modèle, où le préfixe _pk_ses est suivi d'une courte série de chiffres et de lettres, ce qui est considéré comme un code de référence pour le domaine définissant le cookie.
Ce nom de cookie est associé à la plateforme d'analyse Web open source Matomo. Il est utilisé pour aider les propriétaires de sites Web à suivre le comportement des visiteurs et à mesurer les performances du site. Il s'agit d'un cookie de type modèle, où le préfixe _pk_ref est suivi d'une courte série de chiffres et de lettres, ce qui est considéré comme un code de référence pour le domaine définissant le cookie.
Les cookies sont de petits fichiers texte qui sont placés sur votre ordinateur par les sites Web que vous visitez. Les sites Web utilisent des cookies pour aider les utilisateurs à naviguer efficacement et à exécuter certaines fonctions. Les cookies nécessaires au bon fonctionnement du site Web peuvent être configurés sans votre autorisation. Tous les autres cookies doivent être approuvés avant de pouvoir être configurés dans le navigateur.
Vous pouvez modifier votre consentement à l\'utilisation des cookies à tout moment sur notre page Politique de confidentialité.