Child Behavior/physiology; Child, Preschool; Female; Humans; Imitative Behavior/physiology; Male; Group Processes; Social Identification; Social Learning; Trust; Child Behavior; Imitative Behavior; Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health; Education; Developmental and Educational Psychology
Abstract :
[en] Group membership is a strong driver of everyday life in humans, influencing similarity judgments, trust choices, and learning processes. However, its ontogenetic development remains to be understood. This study investigated how group membership, age, sex, and identification with a team influenced 39- to 60-month-old children (N = 94) in a series of similarity, trust, and learning tasks. Group membership had the most influence on similarity and trust tasks, strongly biasing choices toward in-groups. In contrast, prior experience and identification with the team were the most important factors in the learning tasks. Finally, overimitation occurred most when the children's team, but not the opposite, displayed meaningless actions. Future work must investigate how these cognitive abilities combine during development to facilitate cultural processes.
Disciplines :
Social, industrial & organizational psychology
Author, co-author :
Gruber, Thibaud; University of Geneva ; University of Oxford
Deschenaux, Amélie; University of Neuchâtel
Frick, Aurélien ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département des sciences cliniques ; University of Edinburgh ; Suor Orsola Benincasa University of Naples
Clément, Fabrice; University of Neuchâtel
Language :
English
Title :
Group Membership Influences More Social Identification Than Social Learning or Overimitation in Children.
ERC - European Research Council SNF - Schweizerischer Nationalfonds zur Förderung der wissenschaftlichen Forschung
Funding text :
The research leading to these results has received funding from the People Programme (Marie Curie Actions) and from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme for research, technological development, and demonstration under REA grant agreement no. 329197 awarded to Thibaud Gruber. Thibaud Gruber was also supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation during the writing of this article (Grant CR13I1_162720/1). The authors wish to thank the school directors, teachers, parents, and children for participating in the experiments. Thibaud Gruber wishes to thank Christof Neumann and Christine Sievers for discussions, and Brittany Fallon and Cat Hobaiter for English proofreading and discussions about this article. We thank three anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments on previous versions of the article.
Aboud, F. E. (1988). Children and prejudice. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Aboud, F. E. (2003). The formation of in-group favoritism and out-group prejudice in young children: Are they distinct attitudes? Developmental Psychology, 39, 48–60.
Aboud, F. E., & Amato, M. (2001). Developmental and socialization influences on intergroup bias. In R. Brown & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Intergroup processes (pp. 65–85). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Abrams, D., Rutland, A., & Cameron, L. (2003). The development of subjective group dynamics: Children's judgments of normative and deviant in-group and out-group individuals. Child Development, 74, 1840–1856.
Arthur, A. E., Bigler, R. S., Liben, L. S., Gelman, S. A., & Ruble, D. N. (2008). Gender stereotyping and prejudice in young children: A developmental intergroup perspective. In S. Levy & M. Killen (Eds.), Intergroup attitudes and relations in childhood through adulthood (pp. 66–86). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 70, 1–70.
Banaji, M., Baron, A. S., Dunham, Y., & Olson, K. (2008). Some experiments on the development of intergroup social cognition. In S. R. Levy & M. Killen (Eds.), Intergroup relations: An integrative developmental and social psychological perspective (pp. 87–104). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit attitudes. Psychological Science, 17, 53–58.
Bauer, P. J., & Kleinknecht, E. E. (2002). To “ape” or to emulate? Young children's use of both strategies in a single study. Developmental Science, 5, 18–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00197
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
Bennett, M., & Sani, F. (2008). Children's subjective identification with social groups: A group-reference effect approach. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 381–387. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151007x246268
Bernard, S., Mercier, H., & Clément, F. (2012). The power of well-connected arguments: Early sensitivity to the connective “because”. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 128–135.
Bigler, R. S., Spears Brown, C., & Markell, M. (2001). When groups are not created equal: Effects of group status on the formation of intergroup attitudes in children. Child Development, 72, 1151–1162. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00339
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Brewer, M. B. (2008). Depersonalized trust and ingroup cooperation. In J. I. Krueger (Ed.), Rationality and social responsibility: Essays in honor of Robyn Mason Dawes (pp. 215–232). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Brosseau-Liard, P. E., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2014). Sensitivity to confidence cues increases during the second year of life. Infancy, 19, 461–475.
Buttelmann, D., & Bohm, R. (2014). The ontogeny of the motivation that underlies in-group bias. Psychological Science, 25, 921–927.
Buttelmann, D., Zmyj, N., Daum, M. M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-olds. Child Development, 84, 422–428.
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.
Chen, E. E., Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2013). Children trust a consensus composed of outgroup members—But do not retain that trust. Child Development, 84, 269–282. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01850.x
Claidière, N., & Whiten, A. (2012). Integrating the study of conformity and culture in humans and nonhuman animals. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 126–145.
Clément, F., Koenig, M., & Harris, P. L. (2004). The ontogenesis of trust in testimony. Mind and Language, 19, 360–379.
Cook, K. S. (Ed.). (2001). Trust in society. New York, NY: Russell Sage.
Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2009). Choosing your informant: Weighing familiarity and past accuracy. Developmental Science, 12, 426–437.
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 148–153.
Degner, J., & Wentura, D. (2010). Automatic prejudice in childhood and early adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 356–374.
Doebel, S., & Koenig, M. (2016). The emergence of logical inconsistency understanding in young children and its role in epistemic trust. Child Development. Advance online publication.
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82, 793–811. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01577.x
Dunham, Y., Chen, E. E., & Banaji, M. R. (2013). Two signatures of implicit intergroup attitudes developmental invariance and early enculturation. Psychological Science, 24, 860–868.
Fawcett, C. A., & Markson, L. (2010). Similarity predicts liking in 3-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 345–358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.12.002
Flynn, E., & Whiten, A. (2010). Studying children's social learning experimentally in the wild. Learning & Behavior, 38, 284–296.
Gardiner, A. K., Bjorklund, D. F., Greif, M. L., & Gray, S. K. (2012). Choosing and using tools: Prior experience and task difficulty influence preschoolers' tool-use strategies. Cognitive Development, 27, 240–254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.05.001
Gruber, T. (2016). Great apes do not learn novel tool use easily: Conservatism, functional fixedness, or cultural influence? International Journal of Primatology, 37, 296–316. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-016-9902-4
Gruber, T., Zuberbühler, K., Clément, F., & van Schaik, C. P. (2015). Apes have culture but may not know that they do. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 91. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00091
Hamlin, J. K., Mahajan, N., Liberman, Z., & Wynn, K. (2013). Not like me = bad: Infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others. Psychological Science, 24, 589–594.
Harris, P. L. (2012). Trusting what you're told: How children learn from others. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.
Harris, P. L., Corriveau, K., Pasquini, E. S., Koenig, M., Fusaro, M., & Clément, F. (2012). Credulity and the development of selective trust in early childhood. In M. J. Beran, J. L. Brandl, J. Perner, & J. Proust (Eds.), Foundations of metacognition (pp. 193–210). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Haslam, A., Oakes, P., Turner, J., & McGarty, C. (1996). Social identity, self-categorization, and the perceived homogeneity of ingroups and outgroups: The interaction between social motivation and cognition. In R. Sorrentino & E. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: The interpersonal context, Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 182–222). New York, NY: Guilford.
Haun, D. B. M., & Over, H. (2013). Like me: A homophily-based account of human culture. In P. J. Richerson & M. Christiansen (Eds.), Cultural evolution (pp. 75–85). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Haun, D. B. M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Conformity to peer pressure in preschool children. Child Development, 82, 1759–1767.
Hector, A., Von Felten, S., & Schmid, B. (2010). Analysis of variance with unbalanced data: An update for ecology & evolution. Journal of Animal Ecology, 79, 308–316. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01634.x
Hirschfeld, L. A. (1995). Do children have a theory of race? Cognition, 54, 209–252.
Hopper, L. M., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Whiten, A. (2008). Observational learning in chimpanzees and children studied through “ghost” conditions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275, 835–840.
Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan trogiodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164–181.
Jaswal, V. K., & Neely, L. A. (2006). Adults don't always know best: Preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychological Science, 17, 757–758.
Keupp, S., Behne, T., Zachow, J., Kasbohm, A., & Rakoczy, H. (2015). Overimitation is not automatic: Context sensitivity in children's overimitation and action interpretation of causally irrelevant actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 163–175.
Killen, M. (2007). Children's social and moral reasoning about exclusion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 32–36.
Killen, M., & Stangor, C. (2001). Children's social reasoning about inclusion and exclusion in gender and race peer group contexts. Child Development, 72, 174–186.
Kinzler, K. D., Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2011). Children's selective trust in native-accented speakers. Developmental Science, 14, 106–111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00965.x
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 12577–12580.
Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., DeJesus, J., & Spelke, E. S. (2009). Accent trumps race in children's social preferences. Social Cognition, 27, 623–634.
Koenig, M., Clément, F., & Harris, P. L. (2004). Trust in testimony: Children's use of true and false statements. Psychological Science, 10, 694–698.
Kramer, R. M. (1999). Trust and distrust in organizations: Emerging perspectives, enduring questions. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 569–598.
Laland, K. N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Learning & Behavior, 32, 4–14.
Landrum, A. R., Mills, C. M., & Johnston, A. M. (2013). When do children trust the expert? Benevolence information influences children's trust more than expertise. Developmental Science, 16, 622–638. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12059
Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (2013). Informants' traits weigh heavily in young children's trust in testimony and in their epistemic inferences. Child Development, 84, 1253–1268.
Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 19751–19756. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0704452104
Mahajan, N., & Wynn, K. (2012). Origins of “Us” versus “Them”: Prelinguistic infants prefer similar others. Cognition, 124, 227–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.003
Marques, J. M., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Leyens, J.-P. (1988). The black sheep effect: Judgmental extremity towards ingroup members as a function of ingroup identification. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 1–16.
Mascaro, O., & Sperber, D. (2009). The moral, epistemic, and mindreading components of children's vigilance towards deception. Cognition, 112, 367–380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.012
McGuigan, N., Makinson, J., & Whiten, A. (2010). From over-imitation to super copying: Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 1–18.
Mundry, R. (2011). Issues in information theory-based statistical inference—A commentary from a frequentist's perspective. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65, 57–68. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-1040-y
Nesdale, D., & Flesser, D. (2001). Social identity and the development of children's group attitudes. Child Development, 72, 506–517.
Nesdale, D., Maass, A., Griffiths, J., & Durkin, K. (2003). Effects of in-group and out-group ethnicity on children's attitudes towards members of the in-group and out-group. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 177–192.
Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 555–565.
Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010). Overimitation in Kalahari Bushmen and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 5, 729–736.
Oostenbroek, J., & Over, H. (2015). Young children contrast their behavior to that of out-group members. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 139, 234–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.05.009
Over, H., Carpenter, M., Spears, R., & Gattis, M. (2013). Children selectively trust individuals who have imitated them. Social Development, 22, 215–224. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12020
Richter, N., Over, H., & Dunham, Y. (2016). The effects of minimal group membership on young preschoolers' social preferences, estimates of similarity, and behavioral attribution. Collabra, 2, 1–8.
Rutland, A., Killen, M., & Abrams, D. (2010). A new social-cognitive perspective on prejudice: The interplay between morality and group identity. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 279–291.
Sabbagh, M. A., & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers' theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development, 72, 1054–1070. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00334
Sabbagh, M. A., Wdowiak, S. D., & Ottaway, J. M. (2003). Do word learners ignore ignorant speakers? Journal of Child Language, 30, 905–924.
Schielzeth, H. (2010). Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 103–113. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210X.2010.00012.x
Sherif, M. (1937). An experimental approach to the study of attitudes. Sociometry, 1, 90–98.
Sheskin, M., Bloom, P., & Wynn, K. (2014). Anti-equality: Social comparison in young children. Cognition, 130, 152–156.
Taylor, M. J. (2013). Gender influences on children's selective trust of adult testimony. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 672–690.
Terrier, N., Bernard, S., Mercier, H., & Clément, F. (2016). Visual access trumps gender in 3- and 4-year-old children's endorsement of testimony. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 146, 223–230.
Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Tomasello, M. (2009). The question of chimpanzee culture, plus postscript (Chimpanzee culture, 2009). In K. N. Laland & B. G. Galef (Eds.), The question of animal culture (pp. 198–221). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Turner, J. C., & Oakes, P. J. (1986). The significance of the social identity concept for social psychology with reference to individualism, interactionism and social influence. British Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 237–252. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1986.tb00732.x
Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 2417–2428.
Williamson, R. A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). Own and others' prior experiences influence children's imitation of causal acts. Cognitive Development, 26, 260–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.04.002
Wood, L. A., Kendal, R. L., & Flynn, E. G. (2013). Copy me or copy you? The effect of prior experience on social learning. Cognition, 127, 203–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.01.002
Yamagishi, T., & Yamagishi, M. (1994). Trust and commitment in the United States and Japan. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 129–166.
Yuki, M., Maddux, W. W., Brewer, M. B., & Takemura, K. (2005). Cross-cultural differences in relationship-and group-based trust. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 48–62.