Age Factors; Animals; Child; Concept Formation; Female; Humans; Male; Pan troglodytes/psychology; Psychology, Child; Tool Use Behavior; Pan troglodytes; Multidisciplinary
Abstract :
[en] The acquisition of the concept of 'tool' remains intriguing from both developmental and comparative perspectives. Our current model of tool use development in children is based on humans' supposedly unique ability to adopt a teleological stance: the understanding of a demonstrator's goal-based intentions when using a tool. It is however unclear how children and chimpanzees, our closest relatives, combine their knowledge of different objects whose function is to act on other parts of the environment, and assign them to a single category of 'tools'. Here, we used a function-based approach to address this question. We exposed 7 to 11-year-old children and adult chimpanzees to a Matching-to-Function (MTF) task to explore whether they would sort tools and non-tools separately after demonstration of their function by an experimenter. MTF is a variant of Matching-to-Sample where the sample and the target are from the same category/kind rather than identical. Around 40% of children paired objects according to their function in the MTF task, with only one child younger than 8 years doing so. Moreover, when verbally questioned, these children offered a function-based answer to explain their choices. One of six chimpanzees also successfully paired objects according to function. Children and at least one chimpanzee can thus spontaneously sort tools into functional categories based on observing a demonstrator. The success of a single chimpanzee in our task suggests that teleological reasoning might already have been present in our last common ancestor but also shows that human children more readily conceptualize tools in a spontaneous fashion.
Disciplines :
Animal psychology, ethology & psychobiology
Author, co-author :
Gruber, Thibaud ; Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK. thibaud.gruber@unige.ch ; Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland. thibaud.gruber@unige.ch
Frick, Aurélien ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département des sciences cliniques ; Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK ; Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, Suor Orsola Benincasa University, Naples, Italy
Hirata, Satoshi; Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
Adachi, Ikuma; Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Japan
Biro, Dora; Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK. dora.biro@zoo.ox.ac.uk
Language :
English
Title :
Spontaneous categorization of tools based on observation in children and chimpanzees.
SNF - Schweizerischer Nationalfonds zur Förderung der wissenschaftlichen Forschung
Funding text :
We thank all technical staff and caretakers at the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, as well as the school staff and participating children at Ecole Sainte-Croix in Orléans. We are grateful to an anonymous reviewer for comments and suggestions for improving the manuscript. This work was funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant Number P300PA_164678 to TG).
Shumaker, R. W., Walkup, K. R. & Beck, B. B. Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals. (The Johns Hopkins University Press, 2011).
Wynn, T. Archaeology and cognitive evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25, 389–+ (2003).
Goodall, J. Tool-using and aimed throwing in a community of free-living chimpanzees. Nature 201, 1264–1266 (1964).
Tomasello, M. & Call, J. Primate cognition. (Oxford University Press, 1997).
Whiten, A. et al. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399, 682–685 (1999).
Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S. & Hopper, L. M. Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364, 2417–2428 (2009).
Hobaiter, C., Poisot, T., Zuberbühler, K., Hoppitt, W. & Gruber, T. Social network analysis shows direct evidence for social transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees. PLOS Biology 12, e1001960 (2014).
Matsuzawa, T. In Chimpanzee cultures (eds. Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M. & Heltne, P. G.) 351–370 (HUP, 1994).
Lamon, N., Neumann, C., Gier, J., ZUberbühler, K. & Gruber, T. Wild chimpanzees select tool material based on efficiency and knowledge. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences (2018).
Gruber, T., Muller, M. N., Reynolds, V., Wrangham, R. W. & Zuberbühler, K. Community-specific evaluation of tool affordances in wild chimpanzees. Scientific Reports 1, 128, 10.1038/srep00128 (2011).
Visalberghi, E. et al. Selection of effective stone tools by wild bearded capuchin monkeys. Current Biology 19, 213–217 (2009).
Cardoso, R. M. & Ottoni, E. B. The effects of tradition on problem solving by two wild populations of bearded capuchin monkeys in a probing task. Biol Letters, 12, 10.1098/rsbl.2016.0604 (2016).
Zuberbühler, K. & Wittig, R. M. In Field and laboratory methods in primatology: A practical guide (eds. Curtis, D. J. & Setchell, J. M.) 207–224 (Cambridge University Press, 2011).
Boesch, C. What makes us human (Homo sapiens)? The challenge of cognitive cross-species comparison. Journal of Comparative Psychology 121, 227–240 (2007).
Seed, A. M., Call, J., Emery, N. J. & Clayton, N. S. Chimpanzees solve the trap problem when the confound of tool-use is removed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 35, 23–34 (2009).
Tanaka, M. Formation of categories based on functions in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Japanese Psychological Research 39, 212–225 (1997).
Karmiloff-Smith, A. Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. (MIT Press, 1992).
Gruber, T., Zuberbühler, K., Clément, F. & van Schaik, C. P. Apes have culture but may not know that they do. Frontiers in Psychology 6, 91, 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00091 (2015).
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D. M. & Boysen, S. Symbolic communication between two chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Science 201, 641–644 (1978).
Hayes, K. J. & Nissen, C. H. In Behaviour of Non-human Primates Vol. 4 (eds. Schrier, A. M. & Stollnitz, F.) 50–115 (Academic Press, 1971).
Matsuzawa, T. In “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes. Comparative developmental perspectives (eds. Parker, S. T. & Gibson, K. R.) 451–468 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1990).
Epstein, R., Lanza, R. P. & Skinner, B. F. Symbolic communication between two pigeons (Columbia livia domestica). Science 207, 543–545 (1980).
Santos, L. R., Hauser, M. D. & Spelke, E. S. In The Cognitive Animal (eds. Bekoff, M., Allen, C. & Burghardt, G.) 205–215 (MIT Press, 2002).
Hauser, M. D. & Santos, L. R. In Creations of the mind: Theories of artifacts and their representation (eds. Margolis, E. & Laurence, S.) 267–288 (OUP, 2007).
Margolis, E. & Laurence, S. (OUP, Oxford, 2007).
Hood, B. M. & Santos, L. R. (OUP, Oxford, 2009).
Mandler, J. M. In Creations of the mind: Theories of artifacts and their representation (eds. Margolis, E. & Laurence, S.) 191–211 (OUP, 2007).
DiYanni, C. & Kelemen, D. Using a bad tool with good intention: Young children’s imitation of adults’ questionable choices. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 101, 241–261 (2008).
Kelemen, D. & Carey, S. In Creations of the mind: Theories of artifacts and their representation (eds. Margolis, E. & Laurence, S.) 212–230 (OUP, 2007).
Defeyter, M. A. & German, T. P. Acquiring an understanding of design: Evidence from children’s insight problem solving. Cognition 89, 133–155, 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00098-2 (2003).
Hernik, M. & Csibra, G. Functional understanding facilitates learning about tools in human children. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 19, 34–38 (2009).
Ruiz, A. M. & Santos, L. R. In Tool use in animals: Cognition and ecology (eds. Sanz, C. M., Call, J. & Boesch, C.) 119–133 (Cambridge University Press, 2013).
Casler, K. & Kelemen, D. Young children’s rapid learning about artifacts. Developmental Science 8, 472–480, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00438.x (2005).
Tomonaga, M. & Itakura, S. Teaching ordinals to a cardinal trained chimpanzee. Primate Research 9, 67–77 (1993).
Matsuzawa, T. In Cognitive Development in Chimpanzees (eds. Matsuzawa, T., Tomonaga, M. & Tanaka, M.) 3–33 (Springer, 2006).
Matsuzawa, T., Tomonaga, M. & Tanaka, M. (2006).
Matsuzawa, T. The Ai project: historical and ecological contexts. Animal Cognition 6, 199–211 (2003).
Hayashi, M., Takeshita, H., Matsuzawa, T., Tomonaga, M. & Tanaka, M. Cognitive development in apes and humans assessed by object manipulation. Cognitive Development in Chimpanzees., 395–410 (2006).
Hirata, S. & Morimura, N. Naive chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) observation of experienced conspecifics in a tool-using task. Journal of Comparative Psychology 114, 291–296 (2000).
Celli, M. L., Hirata, S. & Tomonaga, M. Socioecological influences on tool use in captive chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology 25, 1267–1281 (2004).
Wynn, T. & McGrew, W. C. An Apes View of the Oldowan. Man 24, 383–398 (1989).
Hernik, M. & Csibra, G. Infants learn enduring functions of novel tools from action demonstrations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 130, 176–192, 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.10.004 (2015).
Gruber, T. Great apes do not learn novel tool use easily: Conservatism, functional fixedness, or cultural influence? International Journal of Primatology 37, 296–316, 10.1007/s10764-016-9902-4 (2016).
German, T. P. & Defeyter, M. A. Immunity to functional fixedness in young children. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 7, 707–712 (2000).
Haun, D. B. M. & Tomasello, M. Conformity to peer pressure in preschool children. Child Development 82, 1759–1767 (2011).
Gruber, T., Deschenaux, A., Frick, A. & Clément, F. Group membership influences more social identification than social learning or overimitation in children. Child Development 90, 728–745, 10.1111/cdev.12931 (2019).
Lonsdorf, E. V., Everly, L. E. & Pusey, A. E. Sex differences in learning in chimpanzees. Nature 428, 715–716 (2004).
Gruber, T., Clay, Z. & Zuberbühler, K. A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage. Animal Behaviour 80, 1023–1033 (2010).
Frick, A., Clément, F. & Gruber, T. Evidence for a sex effect during overimitation: boys copy irrelevant modelled actions more than girls across cultures. Royal Society Open Science, 4, 10.1098/rsos.170367 (2017).
Lamon, N., Neumann, C., Gruber, T. & Zuberbühler, K. Kin-based cultural transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees. Science Advances 3, e1602750 (2017).
Gruber, T. & Sievers, C. In Foundations of Affective Social Learning: Conceptualising the Transmission of Social Value (eds. Dukes, D. & Clément, F.) 41–66 (Cambridge University Press, 2019).