Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. VIII (pp. 47-90). New York: Academic Press.
Besner, D. (1987). Phonology, lexical access in reading, and articulatory suppression: A critical review. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9A, 467-478.
Besner, D., & Davelaar, E. (1982). Basic processes in reading: Two phonological codes. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36(4), 701-711.
Besner, D., Davies, J., & Daniels, S. (1981). Reading for meaning: The effects of concurrent articulation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 415-437.
Best, W., & Howard, D. (2005). "The W and M are mixing me up": Use of a visual code in verbal shortterm memory tasks. Brain & Cognition, 58, 274-285.
Bourassa, D. C., & Besner, D. (1994). Beyond the articulatory loop: A semantic contribution to serial order recall of subspan lists. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 122-125.
Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J. F., & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention & performance VI (pp. 535-555). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
Crowder, R. G. (1978). Memory for phonologically uniform lists. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 73-89.
Fallon, A. B., Groves, K., & Tehan, G. (1999). Phonological similarity and trace degradation in the serial recall task: When CAT helps RAT, but not MAN. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 301-307.
Gupta, P., & MacWhinney, B. (1997). Vocabulary acquisition and verbal short-term memory: Computational and neural bases. Brain and Language, 59, 267-333.
Howard, D., & Best, W. (1996). Developmental phonological dyslexia: Real word reading can be completely normal. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 887-934.
Howard, D., & Nickels, L. (2005). Separating input and output phonology: Semantic, phonological, and orthographic effects in short-term memory impairment. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(1), 42-77.
Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., Brown, G., & Mercer, R. (1995). The role of long-term memory mechanisms in memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 527-536.
McCann, R. S., & Besner, D. (1987). Reading pseudohomophones: Implications for models of pronunciation assembly and the locus of word-frequency effects in naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 14-24.
Martin, R. C., Lesch, M. F., & Bartha, M. C. (1999). Independence of input and output phonology in word processing and short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 3-29.
Nickels, L., Biedermann, B., Coltheart, M., Saunders, S., & Tree, J. J. (2008). Computational modelling of phonological dyslexia: How does the DRC model fare? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 165-193.
Poirier, M., & St Aubin, J. (1995). Memory for related and unrelated words: Further evidence on the influence of semantic factors in immediate serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 384-404.
Roodenrys, S., Hulme, C., Alban, J., Ellis, A. W., & Brown, G. D. (1994). Effects of word frequency and age of acquisition on short-term memory span. Memory and Cognition, 22, 695-701.
Saint-Aubin, J., & Poirier, M. (1999). Semantic similarity and immediate serial recall: Is there a detrimental effect on order information? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 367-394.
Vallar, G., & Baddeley, A.D. (1982). Short-term forgetting and the articulatory loop. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34A, 53-60.
Walker, I., & Hulme, C. (1999). Concrete words are easier to recall than abstract words: Evidence for a semantic contribution to short-term serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1256-1271.