[en] Total locked-in syndrome is characterized by tetraplegia, anarthria and paralysis of eye motility. In this study, consciousness was detected in a 21-year-old woman who presented a total locked-in syndrome after a basilar artery thrombosis (49 days post-injury) using an active event-related paradigm. The patient was presented sequences of names containing the patient's own name and other names. The patient was instructed to count her own name or to count another target name. Similar to 4 age- and gender-matched healthy controls, the P3 response recorded for the voluntarily counted own name was larger than while passively listening. This P3 response was observed 14 days before the first behavioral signs of consciousness. This study shows that our active event-related paradigm allowed to identify voluntary brain activity in a patient who would behaviorally be diagnosed as comatose.
Disciplines :
Neurology
Author, co-author :
Schnakers, Caroline ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Centre de recherches du cyclotron
Perrin, Fabien
Schabus, Manuel
Hustinx, Roland ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Médecine nucléaire
Majerus, Steve ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département des sciences cognitives > Psychopathologie cognitive
F.R.S.-FNRS - Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique French Speaking Community of Belgium Concerted Research Action JSMF - James S McDonnell Foundation EU - European Commission MSF - Mind Science Foundation
American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine (1995). Recommendations for use of uniform nomenclature pertinent to patients with severe alterations of consciousness. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 76, 205-209.
Bauer, G., Gerstenbrand, F., & Rumpl, E. (1979). Varieties of the locked-in syndrome. Journal of Neurology, 221(2), 77-91.
Bruno, M. A., Pellas, F., Schnakers, C., Van Eeckhout, P., Bernheim, J., Pantke, K. H., et al. (2008). [Blink and you live: The locked-in syndrome]. Revue Neurologique (Paris), 164(4), 322-335.
Constantinidis, C., & Procyk, E. (2004). The primate working memory networks. Cognitive Affective & Behavorial Neuroscience, 4(4), 444-465.
Crawford, J. R., & Garthwaite, P. H. (2002). Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: Confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia, 40(8), 1196-1208.
Davis, M. H., Coleman, M. R., Absalom, A. R., Rodd, J. M., Johnsrude, I. S., Matta, B. F., et al. (2007). Dissociating speech perception and comprehension at reduced levels of awareness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(41), 16032-16037.
Giacino, J., Ashwal, S., Childs, N., Cranford, R., Jennett, B., Katz, D. I., et al. (2002). The minimally conscious state: Definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology, 58(3), 349-353.
Giacino, J., Kalmar, K., & Whyte, J. (2004). The JFK Coma Recovery Scale-Revised: Measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 85(12), 2020-2029.
Klem, G. H., Luders, H. O., Jasper, H. H., & Elger, C. (1999). The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology (Suppl.), 52, 3-6.
Laureys, S., Pellas, F., Van Eeckhout, P., Ghorbel, S., Schnakers, C., Perrin, F., et al. (2005). The locked-in syndrome: What is it like to be conscious but paralyzed and voiceless? Progress in Brain Research, 150, 495-511.
Laureys, S., Perrin, F., & Bredart, S. (2007). Self-consciousness in non-communicative patients. Consciousness and Cognition, 16(3), 722-741.
Majerus, S., Gill-Thwaites, H., Andrews, K., & Laureys, S. (2005). Behavioral evaluation of consciousness in severe brain damage. Progress in Brain Research, 150, 397-413.
Onofrj, M., Melchionda, D., Thomas, A., & Fulgente, T. (1996). Reappearance of event-related P3 potential in locked-in syndrome. Cognitive Brain Research, 4(2), 95-97.
Owen, A., Coleman, M., Boly, M., Davis, M. H., Laureys, S., & Pickard, J. (2006). Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science, 313(5792), 1402.
Perrin, F., Garcia-Larrea, L., Mauguiere, F., & Bastuji, H. (1999). A differential brain response to the subject's own name persists during sleep. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110(12), 2153-2164.
Perrin, F., Schnakers, C., Degueldre, C., Schabus, M., Goldman, S., Brédart, S., et al. (2006). Brain response to one's own name in vegetative state, minimally conscious state and locked-in syndrome. Archives of Neurology, 63(4), 562-569.
Plum, F., & Posner, J. B. (1966). The diagnosis of stupor and coma (1st ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Davis, F.A.
Plum, F., & Posner, J. B. (1983). The diagnosis of stupor and coma (3rd ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Davis, F.A.
Richard, I., Pereon, Y., Guiheneu, P., Nogues, B., Perrouin-Verbe, B., & Mathe, J. F. (1995). Persistence of distal motor control in the locked in syndrome. Review of 11 patients. Paraplegia, 33(11), 640-646.
Schnakers, C., Majerus, S., & Laureys, S. (2005). A comparison of behavioural assessement tools and electrophysiological measures of recovery from coma. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 15, 381-388.
Schnakers, C., Perrin, F., Schabus, M., Majerus, S., Ledoux, D., Damas, P., et al. (2008). Voluntary brain processing in disorders of consciousness. Neurology. 71(20), 1614-1620.
Sleigh, J. W., Andrzejowski, J., Steyn-Ross, A., & Steyn-Ross, M. (1999). The bispectral index: A measure of depth of sleep? Anesthesia & Analgesia, 88(3), 659-661.
The Multi-Society Task Force on PVS (1994). Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state (1). New England Journal of Medicine, 330(21), 1499-1508.