Aging; cost-efficiency; learning; memory; naming; Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology; Experimental and Cognitive Psychology; Geriatrics and Gerontology; Psychiatry and Mental Health
Abstract :
[en] People's names are challenging to learn at all ages. Because people somewhat know this, they might spontaneously use cost-efficient encoding strategies and devote more resources to learn names that are most likely to be useful. To test this hypothesis, we created a pseudo-incidental learning situation in which young and older participants were exposed to 12 characters from a TV show and reviewed face-name-instrument triplets. Characters' probability of appearance was specified via importance labels (main or secondary characters, bit parts). A surprised cued recall test showed that young adults performed better than older ones, and that semantic information was better recalled than names. Consistent with cost-efficient encoding strategies, participants in both groups recalled names and semantic information about most important characters better. Interestingly, there were large individual differences: people who reported using cost-efficient strategies performed better. At the individual level, memory advantages for most important characters' names and semantic information correlated.
Disciplines :
Theoretical & cognitive psychology
Author, co-author :
Devue, Christel ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Psychologie > Psychologie et neurosciences cognitives
Badolle, Marie; Psychology and Neuroscience of Cognition Research Unit, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
Brédart, Serge ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Psychologie
Language :
English
Title :
Strategic learning of people's names as a function of expected utility in young and older adults.
Publication date :
29 March 2024
Journal title :
Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition
Adelman, J. S., & Estes, Z., (2013). Emotion and memory: A recognition advantage for positive and negative words independent of arousal. Cognition, 129(3), 530–535. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.014
Anderson, J. R., & Schooler, L. J., (2000). The adaptive nature of memory. In E., Tulving & F., Craik (Eds.), Handbook of memory (pp. 557–570). Oxford University Press.
Barresi, A., Obler, L. K., & Goodglass, H., (1998). Dissociation between proper name and common noun learning. Brain and Cognition, 37, 21–23. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1997.0987
Biss, R. K., Rowe, G., Weeks, J. C., Hasher, L., & Murphy, K. J., (2018). Leveraging older adults’ susceptibility to distraction to improve memory for face-name associations. Psychology and Aging, 33(1), 158–164. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000192
Brédart, S., (2017). The cognitive psychology and neuroscience of naming people. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 83, 145–154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.008
Brédart, S., (2019). Strategies to improve name learning: A review. European Psychologist, 24(4), 349–358. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000363
Brédart, S., & Valentine, T., (1998). Descriptiveness and proper name retrieval. Memory, 6(2), 199–206. https://doi.org/10.1080/741942072
Brennen, T., (1993). The difficulty with recalling people’s names: The plausible phonology hypothesis. Memory, 1, 409–431. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658219308258246
Campbell, K. L., Hasher, L., & Thomas, R. C., (2010). Hyper-binding: A unique age effect. Psychological Science, 21(3), 399–405. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609359910
Castel, A. D., (2007). The adaptive and strategic use of memory by older adults: Evaluative processing and value- directed remembering. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 48, 225–270. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(07)48006-9
Cohen, G., (1990). Why is it difficult to put names to faces?British Journal of Psychology, 81(3), 287–297. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02362.x
Cohen, G., (1994). Age-related problems in the use of proper names in communication. In M. L., Hummert, J. M., Wiemann, & J. F., Nussbaum (Eds.), Interpersonal communication in older adulthood (pp. 40–57). Sage.
Condret-Santi, V., Barbeau, E. J., Matharan, F., Le Goff, M., Dartigues, J.-F., & Amieva, H., (2013). Prevalence of word retrieval complaint and prediction of dementia in a population-based study of elderly subjects. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 35(5–6), 313–324. https://doi.org/10.1159/000342594
Cunningham, S. J., & Turk, D. J., (2017). Editorial: A review of self-processing biases in cognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(6), 987–995. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1276609
Devue, C., & de Sena, S., (2023). The impact of stability in appearance on the development of facial representations. Cognition, 239, 105569. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105569
Devue, C., Wride, A., & Grimshaw, G. M., (2019). New insights on real-world human face recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(6), 994–1007. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000493
Festini, S. B., Hartley, A. A., Tauber, S. K., & Rhodes, M. G., (2013). Assigned value improves memory of proper names. Memory, 21(6), 657–667. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2012.747613
Fogler, K. A., James, L. E., & Crandall, E. A., (2010). How name descriptiveness impacts proper name learning in young and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology & Cognition, 17(5), 505–518. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825580903477245
Halamish, V., & Stern, P., (2022). Motivation-based selective encoding and retrieval. Memory and Cognition, 50(4), 736–750. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-021-01238-2
Hanley, J. R., (2014). Retrieval failures for names of familiar people. In B. L., Schwartz & A. S., Brown (Eds.), Tip-of-the-tongue states and related phenomena (pp. 50–74). Cambridge University Press.
Hargis, M. B., & Castel, A. D., (2017). Younger and older adults’ associative memory for social information: The role of information importance. Psychology and Aging, 32(4), 325–330. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000171
Helder, E., & Shaughnessy, J. J., (2008). Retrieval opportunities while multitasking improve name recall. Memory, 16(8), 896–909. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210802360611
James, L. E., (2004). Meeting Mr. Farmer versus meeting a farmer: Specific effects of aging on learning proper names. Psychology and Aging, 19(3), 515–522. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.3.515
Landauer, T. K., & Bjork, R. A., (1978). Optimum rehearsal patterns and name learning. In M. M., Gruneberg, P. E., Morris, & R. N., Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 625–632). Academic Press.
Lovelace, E. A., & Twohig, P. T., (1990). Healthy older adults’ perceptions of their memory functioning and use of mnemonics. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28(2), 115–118. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03333979
Maylor, E. A., (1997). Proper name retrieval in old age: Converging evidence against disproportionate impairment. Aging, Neuropsychology & Cognition, 4(3), 211–226. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825589708256648
McWeeny, K. H., Young, A. W., Hay, D. C., & Ellis, A. W., (1987). Putting names to faces. British Journal of Psychology, 78(2), 143–149. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1987.tb02235.x
Morey, R. D., (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4(2), 61–64. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.04.2.p061
Morris, P. E., & Fritz, C. O., (2000). The name game: Using retrieval practice to improve the learning of names. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6(2), 124–129. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.6.2.124
Morris, P. E., & Fritz, C. O., (2002). The improved name game: Better use of expanding retrieval practice. Memory, 10(4), 259–266. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210143000371
Morris, P. E., Fritz, C. O., Jackson, L., Nichol, E., & Roberts, E., (2005). Strategies for learning proper names: Expanding retrieval practice, meaning and imagery. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19(6), 779–798. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1115
Murphy, D. H., & Castel, A. D., (2022). The role of attention and ageing in the retrieval dynamics of value-directed remembering. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75(5), 954–968. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218211046612
Naveh-Benjamin, M., Guez, J., Kilb, A., & Reedy, S., (2004). The associative memory deficit of older adults: Further support using face-name associations. Psychology and Aging, 19(3), 541–546. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.3.541
Neuschatz, J. S., Preston, E. L., Toglia, M. P., & Neuschatz, J. S., (2005). Comparison of the efficacy of two name-learning techniques: Expanding rehearsal and face-name imagery. The American Journal of Psychology, 118(1), 79–102. https://www.jstor.org/stable/30039044
Old, S. R., & Naveh-Benjamin, M., (2012). Age differences in memory for names: The effect of prelearned semantic associations. Psychology and Aging, 27(2), 462–473. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025194
Rendell, P. G., Castel, A. D., & Craik, F. I. M., (2005). Memory for proper names in old age: A disproportionate impairment?The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58A(1), 54–71. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980443000188
Stefanidi, A., Ellis, D. M., & Brewer, G. A., (2018). Free recall dynamics in value-directed remembering. Journal of Memory and Language, 100, 18–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.11.004
Tauber, S. K., & Rhodes, M. G., (2010). Metacognitive errors contribute to the difficulty in remembering proper names. Memory, 18(5), 522–532. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2010.481818
Terry, W. S., (1994). On the relative difficulty in recalling names and occupations. The American Journal of Psychology, 107(1), 85–94. https://doi.org/10.2307/1423291
Weaver Cargin, J., Collie, A., Masters, C., & Maruff, P., (2008). The nature of cognitive complaints in healthy older adults with and without objective memory decline. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(2), 245–257. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390701377829
Weeks, J. C., Biss, R. K., Murphy, K. L., & Hasher, L., (2016). Face–name learning in older adults: A benefit of hyper-binding. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23(5), 1559–1565. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1003-z