Attention; Attentional switch; Autobiographical memory; Default mode network; Episodic memory retrieval; Gaze aversion; Mental time travel; Perceptual decoupling; Experimental and Cognitive Psychology; Developmental and Educational Psychology; Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous)
Abstract :
[en] Remembering and mentally reliving yesterday's lunch is a typical example of episodic autobiographical memory retrieval. In the present review, we reappraised the complex cascade of cognitive processes involved in memory retrieval, by highlighting one particular phase that has received little interest so far: attentional switch to memory (ASM). As attention cannot be simultaneously directed toward external stimuli and internal memories, there has to be an attentional switch from the external to the internal world in order to initiate memory retrieval. We formulated hypotheses and developed hypothetical models of both the cognitive and brain processes that accompany ASM. We suggest that gaze aversion could serve as an objective temporal marker of the point at which people switch their attention to memory, and highlight several fields (neuropsychology, neuroscience, social cognition, comparative psychology) in which ASM markers could be essential. Our review thus provides a new framework for understanding the early stages of autobiographical memory retrieval.
Disciplines :
Neurosciences & behavior
Author, co-author :
Servais, Anaïs ; CerCo, CNRS UMR5549-Université de Toulouse, CHU Purpan, Pavillon Baudot, 31052, Toulouse, France. anais.servais@hotmail.com ; ENAC, 7, avenue Edouard Belin, 31055, Toulouse, France. anais.servais@hotmail.com
Hurter, Christophe; ENAC, 7, avenue Edouard Belin, 31055, Toulouse, France
Barbeau, Emmanuel J; CerCo, CNRS UMR5549-Université de Toulouse, CHU Purpan, Pavillon Baudot, 31052, Toulouse, France
Language :
English
Title :
Attentional switch to memory: An early and critical phase of the cognitive cascade allowing autobiographical memory retrieval.
Abeles, D., & Yuval-Greenberg, S. (2017). Just look away: Gaze aversions as an overt attentional disengagement mechanism. Cognition, 168, 99–109. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.021
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron, 65(4), 550–562. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005
Andrist, S., Tan, X. Z., Gleicher, M., & Mutlu, B. (2014). Conversational gaze aversion for humanlike robots. In 9th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), (pp 25–32).
Baars, B. (1997). In the theatre of consciousness: The workplace of the mind. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4(4), 292–309.
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423. DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2
Bae, G. Y., & Luck, S. J. (2017). Interactions between visual working memory representations. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 79(8), 2376–2395. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-017-1404-8
Bae, G. Y., & Luck, S. J. (2019). Reactivation of Previous Experiences in a Working Memory Task. Psychological Science, 30(4), 587–595. DOI: 10.1177/0956797619830398
Barbeau, E. J., Chauvel, P., Moulin, C. J. A., Regis, J., & Liégeois-Chauvel, C. (2017). Hippocampus duality: Memory and novelty detection are subserved by distinct mechanisms. Hippocampus, 27(4), 405–416. DOI: 10.1002/hipo.22699
Baron-Cohen, S., & Cross, P. (1992). Reading the eyes: Evidence for the role of perception in the development of a theory of mind. Mind & Language, 7(1/2), 172–186. DOI: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.1992.tb00203.x
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 241–251. DOI: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715
Barzykowski, K., & Moulin, C. (2022). Are involuntary autobiographical memory and déjà vu natural products of memory retrieval? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(3), 615–616.
Bastin, C. (2018). Le réseau cérébral par défaut: Un repos qui n’en est pas un [The default brain network: A rest that is not a rest]. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 10(3), 232–238.
Bastin, C., Besson, G., Simon, J., Delhaye, E., Geurten, M., Willems, S., & Salmon, E. (2019). An integrative memory model of recollection and familiarity to understand memory deficits. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, Article e281.
Benedek, M., Stoiser, R., Walcher, S., & Körner, C. (2017). Eye behavior associated with internally versus externally directed cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1092. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01092
Berntsen, D. (2010). The unbidden past: Involuntary autobiographical memories as a basic mode of remembering. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 138–142. DOI: 10.1177/0963721410370301
Bone, M. B., St-Laurent, M., Dang, C., McQuiggan, D. A., Ryan, J. D., & Buchsbaum, B. R. (2019). Eye movement reinstatement and neural reactivation during mental imagery. Cerebral Cortex, 29(3), 1075–1089.
Bréchet, L., Brunet, D., Birot, G., Gruetter, R., Michel, C. M., & Jorge, J. (2019). Capturing the spatiotemporal dynamics of self-generated, task-initiated thoughts with EEG and fMRI. NeuroImage, 194, 82–92. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.029
Breeden, P., Dere, D., Zlomuzica, A., & Dere, E. (2016). The mental time travel continuum: On the architecture, capacity, versatility and extension of the mental bridge into the past and future. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 27(4), 421–434. DOI: 10.1515/revneuro-2015-0053
Brincat, S. L., & Miller, E. K. (2016). Prefrontal cortex networks shift from external to internal modes during learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(37), 9739–9754. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0274-16.2016
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38. DOI: 10.1196/annals.1440.011
Buckner, R. L., & DiNicola, L. M. (2019). The brain’s default network: Updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20(10), 593–608. DOI: 10.1038/s41583-019-0212-7
Burgess, P. W., Dumontheil, I., & Gilbert, S. J. (2007). The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 290–298. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.004
Burke, J. F., Sharan, A. D., Sperling, M. R., Ramayya, A. G., Evans, J. J., Healey, M. K., Beck, E. N., Davis, K. A., Lucas, T. H., & Kahana, M. J. (2014). Theta and high-frequency activity mark spontaneous recall of episodic memories. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(34), 11355–11365. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2654-13.2014
Cabeza, R., Ciaramelli, E., Olson, I. R., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). The parietal cortex and episodic memory: An attentional account. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(8), 613–625. DOI: 10.1038/nrn2459
Cabeza, R., Mazuz, Y. S., Stokes, J., Kragel, J. E., Woldorff, M. G., Ciaramelli, E., Olson, I. R., & Moscovitch, M. (2011). Overlapping parietal activity in memory and perception: Evidence for the attention to memory model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3209–3217. DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00065
Cabeza, R., & Moscovitch, M. (2013). Memory systems, processing modes, and components: Functional neuroimaging evidence. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 8(1), 49–55. DOI: 10.1177/1745691612469033
Cabeza, R., & St Jacques, P. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 219–227. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.005
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 718–731. DOI: 10.1038/nrn.2016.113
Christoff, K., Ream, J. M., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2004). Neural basis of spontaneous thought processes. Cortex, 40, 623–630.
Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 73–101. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100427
Chun, M. M., & Johnson, M. K. (2011). Memory: Enduring traces of perceptual and reflective attention. Neuron, 72, 520–535. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.026
Ciaramelli, E., Grady, C. L., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: A hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 1828–1851. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.022
Cole, S., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2019). Spontaneous future cognition: The past, present and future of an emerging topic. Psychological Research, 83(4), 631–650. DOI: 10.1007/s00426-019-01193-3
Conway, M. A. (2001). Sensory-perceptual episodic memory and its context: Autobiographical memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 356(1413), 1375–1384. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0940
Conway, M. A. (2009). Episodic memories. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2305–2313. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.003
Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107(2), 261–288. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.261
Conway, M. A., Pleydell-Pearce, C. W., & Whitecross, S. E. (2001). The neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: A slow cortical potential study of autobiographical memory retrieval. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(3), 493–524. DOI: 10.1006/jmla.2001.2781
Conway, M. A., Pleydell-Pearce, C. W., Whitecross, S. E., & Sharpe, H. (2003). Neurophysiological correlates of memory for experienced and imagined events. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 334–340. DOI: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00165-3
Conti, F., & Irish, M. (2021). Harnessing visual imagery and oculomotor behaviour to understand prospection. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(4), 272–283. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.01.009
Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215.
Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford University Press.
Crystal, J. D., & Suddendorf, T. (2019). Episodic memory in nonhuman animals? Current Biology, 29(24), 1291–1295. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.10.045
Dark, V. J. (1990). Switching between memory and perception: Moving attention or memory retrieval? Memory & Cognition, 18(2), 119–127. DOI: 10.3758/BF03197088
Daselaar, S. M., Rice, H. J., Greenberg, D. L., Cabeza, R., LaBar, K. S., & Rubin, D. C. (2008). The spatiotemporal dynamics of autobiographical memory: neural correlates of recall, emotional intensity, and reliving. Cerebral Cortex, 18(1), 217–229.
Denkova, E., Nomi, J. S., Uddin, L. Q., & Jha, A. P. (2019). Dynamic brain network configurations during rest and an attention task with frequent occurrence of mind wandering. Human Brain Mapping, 40(15), 4564–4576. DOI: 10.1002/hbm.24721
Dixon, M. L., Fox, K. C. R., & Christoff, K. (2014). A framework for understanding the relationship between externally and internally directed cognition. Neuropsychologia, 62, 321–330. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.05.024
Doherty-Sneddon, G., & Phelps, F. G. (2005). Gaze aversion: A response to cognitive or social difficulty? Memory & Cognition, 33(4), 727–733. DOI: 10.3758/BF03195338
Droulers, O., & Adil, S. (2015). Perceived gaze direction modulates ad memorization. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 8(1), 15–26. DOI: 10.1037/npe0000029
Einstein, G. O., Earles, J. L., & Collins, H. M. (2002). Gaze aversion: Spared inhibition for visual distraction in older adults. Journals of Gerontology—Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57(1), 65–73. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/57.1.P65
Ellamil, M., Fox, K. C. R., Dixon, M. L., Pritchard, S., Todd, R. M., Thompson, E., & Christoff, K. (2016). Dynamics of neural recruitment surrounding the spontaneous arising of thoughts in experienced mindfulness practitioners. NeuroImage, 136(2016), 186–196. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.034
Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: The neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(6), 581–604. DOI: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00025-7
Fernandes, M. A., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). Factors modulating the effect of divided attention during retrieval of words. Memory & Cognition, 30(5), 731–744. DOI: 10.3758/BF03196429
Fornito, A., Harrison, B. J., Zalesky, A., & Simons, J. S. (2012). Competitive and cooperative dynamics of large-scale brain functional networks supporting recollection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(31), 12788–12793. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1204185109
Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., Ellamil, M., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Christoff, K. (2015). The wandering brain: Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. NeuroImage, 111, 611–621. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.039
Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(27), 9673–9678. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102
Frankland, P. W., Josselyn, S. A., & Köhler, S. (2019). The neurobiological foundation of memory retrieval. Nature Neuroscience, 22(10), 1576–1585. DOI: 10.1038/s41593-019-0493-1
Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Gaze cueing of attention: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 694–724. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.694
Galéra, C., Orriols, L., M’Bailara, K., Laborey, M., Contrand, B., Ribéreau-Gayon, R., Masson, F., Bakiri, S., Gabaude, C., Fort, A., Maury, B., Lemercier, C., Cours, M., Bouvard, M. P., & Lagarde, E. (2012). Mind wandering and driving: Responsibility case-control. BMJ (Online), 345(7888), 10–12.
Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2005). Involvement of rostral prefrontal cortex in selection between stimulus-oriented and stimulus-independent thought. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(5), 1423–1431. DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03981.x
Gilbert, S. J., & Shallice, T. (2002). Task switching: A PDP model. Cognitive Psychology, 44(3), 297–337. DOI: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0770
Gilboa, A. (2004). Autobiographical and episodic memory—One and the same ? Evidence from prefrontal activation in neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia, 42(10), 1336–1349. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.014
Glenberg, A. M., Schroeder, J. L., & Robertson, D. A. (1998). Averting the gaze disengages the environment and facilitates remembering. Memory & Cognition, 26(4), 651–658. DOI: 10.3758/BF03211385
Gorgolewski, K. J., Lurie, D., Urchs, S., Kipping, J. A., Craddock, R. C., Milham, M. P., Margulies, D. S., & Smallwood, J. (2014). A correspondence between individual differences in the brain’s intrinsic functional architecture and the content and form of self-generated thoughts. PLOS ONE, 9(5).
Goulden, N., Khusnulina, A., Davis, N. J., Bracewell, R. M., Bokde, A. L., McNulty, J. P., & Mullins, P. G. (2014). The salience network is responsible for switching between the default mode network and the central executive network: Replication from DCM. NeuroImage, 99, 180–190. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.052
Greenwood, P. M., Blumberg, E. J., & Scheldrup, M. R. (2018). Hypothesis for cognitive effects of transcranial direct current stimulation: Externally- and internally-directed cognition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 86, 226–238. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.006
Handy, T. C., & Kam, J. W. Y. (2015). Mind wandering and selective attention to the external world. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 183–189. DOI: 10.1037/cep0000051
Hautekiet, C., Verschooren, S., Langerock, N., & Vergauwe, E. (2022). Attentional switching between perception and memory: Examining asymmetrical switch costs. PsyArXiv.
Henson, R. N. A., Shallice, T., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). Right prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval: A functional MRI test of the monitoring hypothesis. Brain, 122(7), 1367–1381. DOI: 10.1093/brain/122.7.1367
Herron, J. E., & Evans, L. H. (2018). Preparation breeds success: Brain activity predicts remembering. Cortex, 106, 1–11. DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.009
Huijbers, W., Pennartz, C. M., Cabeza, R., & Daselaar, S. M. (2009). When learning and remembering compete: A functional MRI study. PLOS Biology, 7(1), e11. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000011
Inman, C. S., James, G. A., Vytal, K., & Hamann, S. (2018). Dynamic changes in large-scale functional network organization during autobiographical memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 110, 208–224. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.09.020
James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology (vol. 1). New York: Henry Holt and Co.
Johansson, R., Nyström, M., Dewhurst, R., & Johansson, M. (2022). Eye-movement replay supports episodic remembering. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 289(1976), 20220964.
Johnson, M. R., & Johnson, M. K. (2014). Decoding individual natural scene representations during perception and imagery. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–14. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00059
Kam, J. W. Y., Solbakk, A. K., Endestad, T., Meling, T. R., & Knight, R. T. (2018). Lateral prefrontal cortex lesion impairs regulation of internally and externally directed attention. NeuroImage, 175, 91–99. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.063
Karapanagiotidis, T., Bernhardt, B. C., Jefferies, E., & Smallwood, J. (2017). Tracking thoughts: Exploring the neural architecture of mental time travel during mind-wandering. NeuroImage, 147(6), 272–281. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.031
Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330(6006), 932. DOI: 10.1126/science.1192439
Kim, H. (2015). Encoding and retrieval along the long axis of the hippocampus and their relationships with dorsal attention and default mode networks: The HERNET model. Hippocampus, 25(4), 500–510. DOI: 10.1002/hipo.22387
Kim, H. (2018). Parietal control network activation during memory tasks may be associated with the co-occurrence of externally and internally directed cognition: A cross-function meta-analysis. Brain Research, 1683, 55–66. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2018.01.022
Koike, T., Tanabe, H. C., Okazaki, S., Nakagawa, E., Sasaki, A. T., Shimada, K., Sugawara, S. K., Takahashi, H. K., Yoshihara, K., Bosch-Bayard, J., & Sadato, N. (2016). Neural substrates of shared attention as social memory: A hyperscanning functional magnetic resonance imaging study. NeuroImage, 125, 401–412. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.076
Küper, K. (2018). On the functional significance of retrieval mode: Task switching disrupts the recollection of conceptual stimulus information from episodic memory. Brain Research, 1678, 1–11. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.09.032
Land, M. F., & Tatler, B. W. (2009). Looking and acting: Vision and eye movements in natural behaviour. Oxford University Press.
Lenoble, Q, Janssen, S. M. J., & El Haj, M. (2019). Don‘t stare, unless you don’t want to remember: Maintaining fixation compromises autobiographical memory retrieval. Memory, 27(2), 231–238.
Long, N. M., Kuhl, B. A., & Chun, M. M. (2018). Memory and attention. In Handbook of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience.
Marchetti, G. (2014). Attention and working memory: Two basic mechanisms for constructing temporal experiences. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–15. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00880
Margulies, D. S., Ghosh, S. S., Goulas, A., Falkiewicz, M., Huntenburg, J. M., Langs, G., Bezgin, G., Eickhoff, S. B., Castellanos, F. X., Petrides, M., Jefferies, E., & Smallwood, J. (2016). Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(44), 12574–12579. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1608282113
Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 19(315), 393–395. DOI: 10.1126/science.1131295
Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2000). Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 26(5), 1124–1140.
Mazzoni, G., Clark, A., De Bartolo, A., Guerrini, C., Nahouli, Z., Duzzi, D., De Marco, M., McGeown, W., & Venneri, A. (2019). Brain activation in highly superior autobiographical memory: The role of the praecuneus in the autobiographical memory retrieval network. Cortex, 120, 588–602. DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.02.020
McCormick, C., St-Laurent, M., Ty, A., Valiante, T. A., & McAndrews, M. P. (2015). Functional and effective hippocampal-neocortical connectivity during construction and elaboration of autobiographical memory retrieval. Cerebral Cortex, 25(5), 1297–1305. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bht324
Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5/6), 655–667. DOI: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0
Mildner, J. N., & Tamir, D. I. (2019). Spontaneous thought as an unconstrained memory process. Trends in Neurosciences, 42(11), 763–777. DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2019.09.001
Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140. DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7
Moscovitch, M. (1995). Recovered consciousness: A hypothesis concerning modularity and episodic memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 276-290.
Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (2002). The frontal cortex and working with memory. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 188–209). Oxford University Press.
Moulin, C., Souchay, C. (2013). Epistemic feelings and memory, in: Perfect, T.J., Lindsay, D. S. (Eds.), Sage Handbook of Applied Memory. SAGE Publications Ltd.
Murphy, C., Jefferies, E., Rueschemeyer, S. A., Sormaz, M., Wang, H. T., Margulies, D. S., & Smallwood, J. (2018). Distant from input: Evidence of regions within the default mode network supporting perceptually-decoupled and conceptually-guided cognition. NeuroImage, 171, 393–401. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.017
Nakashima, R., & Shioiri, S. (2014). Why do we move our head to look at an object in our peripheral region? Lateral viewing interferes with attentive search. PLOS ONE, 9(3), e92284. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092284
Nummenmaa, L., & Calder, A. J. (2009). Neural mechanisms of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(3), 135–143. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.12.006
O’Callaghan, C., Shine, J. M., Hodges, J. R., & Irish, M. (2019). Hippocampal atrophy and intrinsic brain network dysfunction relate to alterations in mind wandering in neurodegeneration. Biological Sciences, 116(8), 3316–3321.
Pasupathi, M. (2001). The social construction of the personal past and its implications for adult development. Psychological Bulletin, 127(5), 651–672. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.5.651
Perfect, T. J., Wagstaff, G. F., Moore, D., Andrews, B., Cleveland, V., Newcombe, S., Brisbane, K. A., & Brown, L. (2008). How can we help witnesses to remember more? It’s an (eyes) open and shut case. Law and Human Behavior, 32(4), 314–324. DOI: 10.1007/s10979-007-9109-5
Perrin, D., & Michaelian, K. (2017). Memory as mental time travel. In S. Bernecker & K. Michaelian (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of the philosophy of memory (pp. 228–239). Routledge.
Perrin, D., Michaelian, K., & Sant'Anna, A. (2020). The phenomenology of remembering is an epistemic feeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1531. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01531
Plimpton, B., Patel, P., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2015). Role of triggers and dysphoria in mind-wandering about past, present and future: A laboratory study. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 261–276. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.01.014
Radel, R., & Fournier, M. (2017). The influence of external stimulation in missing knowledge retrieval. Memory, 25(9), 1217–1224. DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1282519
Renoult, L., Irish, M., Moscovitch, M., & Rugg, M. D. (2019). From knowing to remembering: The semantic-episodic distinction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(12), 1041–1057. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.09.008
Rubin, D. C. (2006). The basic-systems model of episodic memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(4), 277–311. DOI: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00017.x
Rubin, D. C., & Greenberg, D. L. (1998). Visual memory-deficit amnesia: A distinct amnesic presentation and etiology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(9), 5413–5416. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.95.9.5413
Rugg, M. D., & Wilding, E. L. (2000). Retrieval processing and episodic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(3), 108–115. DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01445-5
Salvi, C., & Bowden, E. M. (2016). Looking for creativity: Where do we look when we look for new ideas? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 161. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00161
Schnider, A. (2003). Spontaneous confabulation and the adaptation of thought to ongoing reality. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(8), 662–671. DOI: 10.1038/nrn1179
Semon, R. (1921). Le Mneme. London: George Allen and Unwin.
Servais, A., Hurter, C., & Barbeau, E. J. (2022). Gaze direction as a facial cue of memory retrieval state. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1063228. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1063228
Servais, A., Préa, N., Christophe, H., & Barbeau, E. (2023). Why and when do you look away when trying to remember? Gaze aversion as a marker of the attentional switch to the internal world. PsyArXiV.
Sestieri, C., Shulman, G. L., & Corbetta, M. (2010). Attention to memory and the environment: Functional specialization and dynamic competition in human posterior parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(25), 8445–8456. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4719-09.2010
Shapira-Lichter, I., Oren, N., Jacob, Y., Gruberger, M., & Hendler, T. (2013). Portraying the unique contribution of the default mode network to internally driven mnemonic processes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(13), 4950–4955. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1209888110
Shaw, S. B., McKinnon, M. C., Heisz, J., & Becker, S. (2021). Dynamic task-linked switching between brain networks—A tri-network perspective. Brain and Cognition, 151.
Sheldon, S., Cool, K., & El-Asmar, N. (2019). The processes involved in mentally constructing event- and scene-based autobiographical representations. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 31(3), 261–275. DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2019.1614004
Smallwood, J., Beach, E., Schooler, J. W., & Handy, T. C. (2008). Going AWOL in the brain: Mind wandering reduces cortical analysis of external events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(3), 458–469. DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20037
Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946
Smallwood, J., Schooler, J., & Barbara, S. (2015). The science of mind wandering : Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 487–518.
Smallwood, J., Tipper, C., Brown, K., Baird, B., Engen, H., Michaels, J. R., Grafton, S., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Escaping the here and now: Evidence for a role of the default mode network in perceptually decoupled thought. NeuroImage, 69, 120–125. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.012
Sridharan, D., Levitin, D. J., & Menon, V. (2008). A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(34), 12569–12574. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0800005105
Suddendorf, T. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(3), 299–313. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X07001975
Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M. C., & Levine, B. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 2189–2208. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.023
Tang, Y.-Y., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2012). Neural correlates of establishing, maintaining, and switching brain states. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(6), 330–337. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.05.001
Tarder-Stoll, H., Jayakumar, M., Dimsdale-Zucker, H. R., Günseli, E., & Aly, M. (2020). Dynamic internal states shape memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 138, 107328. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107328
Treder, M. S., Charest, I., Michelmann, S., Martin-Buro, M. C., Roux, F., Carceller-Benito, F., Ugalde-Canitrot, A., Rollings, D. T., Sawlani, V., Chelvarajah, R., Wimber, M., Hanslmayr, S., & Staresina, B. P. (2021). The hippocampus as the switchboard between perception and memory. In Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 118(50), e2114171118.
Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson, Organization of memory. Academic Press.
Tulving, E. (1976). Ecphoric processes in recall and recognition. In J. Brown (Ed.), Recall and recognition. John Wiley & Sons.
Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Reviews, 53, 1–25.
Tulving, E., Schacter, D. L., McLachlan, D. R., & Moscovitch, M. (1988). Priming of semantic autobiographical knowledge: A case study of retrograde amnesia. Brain and Cognition, 8(1), 3–20. DOI: 10.1016/0278-2626(88)90035-8
Turkstra, L. S. (2005). Looking while listening and speaking: Eye-to-face gaze in adolescents with and without traumatic brain injury. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(6), 1429–1441. DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/099)
Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Demertzi, A., Schabus, M., Noirhomme, Q., Bredart, S., Boly, M., Phillips, C., Soddu, A., Luxen, A., Moonen, G., & Laureys, S. (2011). Two distinct neuronal networks mediate the awareness of environment and of self. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(3), 570–578. DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21488
Verschooren, S., Liefooghe, B., Brass, M., & Pourtois, G. (2019a). Attentional flexibility is imbalanced: Asymmetric cost for switches between external and internal attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(10), 1399–1414.
Verschooren, S., Schindler, S., De Raedt, R., & Pourtois, G. (2019b). Switching attention from internal to external information processing: A review of the literature and empirical support of the resource sharing account. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(2), 468–490. DOI: 10.3758/s13423-019-01568-y
Verschooren, S., Schindler, S., De Raedt, R., & Pourtois, G. (2021). Early reduction of sensory processing within the visual cortex when switching from internal to external attention. Biological Psychology, 163, 108119. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108119
Vredeveldt, A., Hitch, G. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2011). Eyeclosure helps memory by reducing cognitive load and enhancing visualisation. Memory & Cognition, 39(7), 1253–1263. DOI: 10.3758/s13421-011-0098-8
Vredeveldt, A., Tredoux, C. G., Nortje, A., Kempen, K., Puljevic, C., & Labuschagne, G. N. (2015). A field evaluation of the Eye-Closure Interview with witnesses of serious crimes. Law and Human Behavior, 39(2), 189–197. DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000113
Wais, P. E., Rubens, M. T., Boccanfuso, J., & Gazzaley, A. (2010). Neural mechanisms underlying the impact of visual distraction on retrieval of long-term memory. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 30(25), 8541–8550. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1478-10.2010
Weber, R. J., Burt, D. B., & Noll, N. C. (1986). Attention switching between perception and memory. Memory & Cognition, 14(3), 238–245. DOI: 10.3758/BF03197699
Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2000). Memory’s echo: Vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(20), 11125–11129. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.97.20.11125
Wheeler, M.A., Stuss, D.T., Tulving, E. (1995). Frontal lobe damage produces episodic memory impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1, 525–536.
Wynn, J. S., Shen, K., & Ryan, J. D. (2019). Eye movements actively reinstate spatiotemporal mnemonic content. Vision (Switzerland), 3(2), 1–19.
Xia, J., & Evans, L. H. (2020). Neural evidence that disengaging memory retrieval is modulated by stimulus valence and rumination. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1–11. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-64404-7
Zabelina, D. L., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Dynamic network interactions supporting internally-oriented cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 40, 86–93. DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.06.014
Zeidman, P., & Maguire, E. A. (2016). Anterior hippocampus: The anatomy of perception, imagination and episodic memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(3), 173–182. DOI: 10.1038/nrn.2015.24