Coordinate and categorical relations; Euclidean diagrams; Exact and co-exact relations; Metric and topological relations; Spatial relation processing; Adult; Female; Humans; Male; Photic Stimulation; Reaction Time; Task Performance and Analysis; Visual Fields; Young Adult; Cognition; Dominance, Cerebral; Space Perception; Experimental and Cognitive Psychology; Developmental and Educational Psychology; Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous); General Medicine
Abstract :
[en] The cognitive processing of spatial relations in Euclidean diagrams is central to the diagram-based geometric practice of Euclid's Elements. In this study, we investigate this processing through two dichotomies among spatial relations-metric vs topological and exact vs co-exact-introduced by Manders in his seminal epistemological analysis of Euclid's geometric practice. To this end, we carried out a two-part experiment where participants were asked to judge spatial relations in Euclidean diagrams in a visual half field task design. In the first part, we tested whether the processing of metric vs topological relations yielded the same hemispheric specialization as the processing of coordinate vs categorical relations. In the second part, we investigated the specific performance patterns for the processing of five pairs of exact/co-exact relations, where stimuli for the co-exact relations were divided into three categories depending on their distance from the exact case. Regarding the processing of metric vs topological relations, hemispheric differences were found for only a few of the stimuli used, which may indicate that other processing mechanisms might be at play. Regarding the processing of exact vs co-exact relations, results show that the level of agreement among participants in judging co-exact relations decreases with the distance from the exact case, and this for the five pairs of exact/co-exact relations tested. The philosophical implications of these empirical findings for the epistemological analysis of Euclid's diagram-based geometric practice are spelled out and discussed.
Disciplines :
Theoretical & cognitive psychology
Author, co-author :
Hamami, Yacin ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Traverses ; Centre for Logic and Philosophy of Science, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium. Electronic address: yacin.hamami@gmail.com
van der Kuil, Milan N A; Department Health, Medical and Neuropsychology, Leiden University, Leiden, the Netherlands
Mumma, John; Philosophy Department, California State University of San Bernardino, San Bernardino, CA, USA
van der Ham, Ineke J M; Department Health, Medical and Neuropsychology, Leiden University, Leiden, the Netherlands
Language :
English
Title :
Cognitive Processing of Spatial Relations in Euclidean Diagrams.
This study was partly funded from a Strategic Research Project ( SRP ) of the Vrije Universiteit Brussel (Free University Brussels). YH acknowledges support from a postdoctoral fellowship of the FWO (Research Foundation - Flanders). This study was partly funded from a Strategic Research Project
(SRP) of the Vrije Universiteit Brussel (Free University Brussels). YH
acknowledges support from a postdoctoral fellowship of the FWO
(Research Foundation - Flanders).
Bryant, D.J., Tversky, B., Mental representations of perspective and spatial relations from diagrams and models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 25:1 (1999), 137–156.
Byrne, R.M., Johnson-Laird, P., Spatial reasoning. Journal of Memory and Language 28:5 (1989), 564–575.
Cohn, A.G., Hazarika, S.M., Qualitative spatial representation and reasoning: An overview. Fundamenta Informaticae 46:1–2 (2001), 1–29.
Coventry, K.R., Garrod, S.C., Saying, seeing, and acting: The psychological semantics of spatial prepositions. 2004, Psychology Press, Hove.
Egenhofer, M.J., Franzosa, R.D., Point-set topological spatial relations. International Journal of Geographical Information Systems 5:2 (1991), 161–174.
Euclid, Elements, 1959, Dover Books, New York.
Ferreirós, J., Mathematical knowledge and the interplay of practices. 2015, Princeton University Press, Princeton.
Franklin, N., Tversky, B., Coon, V., Switching points of view in spatial mental models. Memory & Cognition 20:5 (1992), 507–518.
Freeman, J., The modelling of spatial relations. Computer Graphics and Image Processing 4:2 (1975), 156–171.
Giaquinto, M., Visual thinking in mathematics: An epistemological study. 2007, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Hartshorne, R., Geometry: Euclid and beyond. 2000, Springer-Verlag, New York.
Hartshorne, R., Teaching geometry according to Euclid. Notices of the American Mathematical Society 47:4 (2000), 460–465.
Hayward, W.G., Tarr, M.J., Spatial language and spatial representation. Cognition 55:1 (1995), 39–84.
Kelleher, J.D., Costello, F.J., Applying computational models of spatial prepositions to visually situated dialog. Computational Linguistics 35:2 (2009), 271–306.
Knauff, M., Space to reason: A spatial theory of human thought. 2013, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Koedinger, K.R., Anderson, J.R., Abstract planning and perceptual chunks: Elements of expertise in geometry. Cognitive Science 14:4 (1990), 511–550.
Kosslyn, S., Koenig, O., Barrett, A., Backer Cave, C., Tang, J., Gabrieli, J., Evidence for two types of spatial representations: Hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15:4 (1989), 723–735.
Kosslyn, S.M., Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: A computational approach. Psychological Review 94:2 (1987), 148–175.
Kuipers, B., The “map in the head” metaphor. Environment and Behavior 14:2 (1982), 202–220.
Kuipers, B., The cognitive map: Could it have been any other way?. Pick, H.L. Jr., Acredolo, L.P., (eds.) Spatial orientation: Theory, research, and application, 1983, Springer, Boston, MA, 345–359.
Landau, B., Jackendoff, R., “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16:2 (1993), 217–238.
Landy, D., Goldstone, R.L., How abstract is symbolic thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33:4 (2007), 720–733.
Landy, D., Goldstone, R.L., Proximity and precedence in arithmetic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63:10 (2010), 1953–1968.
Mancosu, P., The philosophy of mathematical practice. 2008, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Manders, K., The Euclidean diagram. Mancosu, P., (eds.) The philosophy of mathematical practice, 2008, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 80–133.
Mueller, I., Philosophy of mathematics and deductive structure in Euclid's Elements. 1981, The MIT Press, Cambridge MA.
Munnich, E., Landau, B., Dosher, B.A., Spatial language and spatial representation: A cross-linguistic comparison. Cognition 81:3 (2001), 171–208.
Netz, R., The shaping of deduction in Greek mathematics. 1999, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Randell, D.A., Cui, Z., Cohn, A.G., A spatial logic based on regions and connection. Proceedings of the third international conference on knowledge representation and reasoning, 1992, Morgan Kaufmann, San Mateo, CA, 165–176.
Retz-Schmidt, G., Various views on spatial prepositions. AI Magazine 9:2 (1988), 95–105.
Stevens, A., Coupe, P., Distortions in judged spatial relations. Cognitive Psychology 10:4 (1978), 422–437.
Tversky, B., Cognitive maps, cognitive collages, and spatial mental models. Spatial information theory a theoretical basis for GIS COSIT 1993. Lecture notes in computer science, Vol. 716, 1993, Springer, Berlin, 14–24.
van der Ham, I., Postma, A., Laeng, B., Lateralized perception: The role of attention in spatial relation processing. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 45 (2014), 142–148.
Winston, M.E., Chaffin, R., Herrmann, D., A taxonomy of part-whole relations. Cognitive Science 11:4 (1987), 417–444.
Winston, P.H., Learning structural descriptions from examples. AI Technical Report AITR-231, TR-76, 1970, MIT.