Abrahamse, E., Van Dijck, J., Majerus, S., & Fias, W. (2014). Finding the answer in space: The mental whiteboard hypothesis on serial order in working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 932. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00932
Acheson, D. J., & MacDonald, M. C. (2009). Verbal working memory and language production: Common approaches to the serial ordering of verbal information. Psychological Bulletin, 135(1), 50–68. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014411
Allen, R., & Hulme, C. (2006). Speech and language processing mechanisms in verbal serial recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(1), 64–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.02.002
Attout, L., Fias, W., Salmon, E., & Majerus, S. (2014). Common neural substrates for ordinal representation in short-term memory, numerical and alphabetical cognition. PLoS ONE, 9(3), Article e92049. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092049
Baguley, T. (2012). Calculating and graphing within-subject confidence intervals for ANOVA. Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 158–175. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0123-7
Botvinick, M. M., & Plaut, D. C. (2006). Short-term memory for serial order: A recurrent neural network model. Psychological Review, 113(2), 201–233. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.201
Bourassa, D. C., & Besner, D. (1994). Beyond the articulatory loop: A semantic contribution to serial order recall of subspan lists. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(1), 122–125. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200768
Brener, R. (1940). An experimental investigation of memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 26(5), 467–482. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0061096
Brock, J., & Jarrold, C. (2005). Serial order reconstruction in Down syndrome: Evidence for a selective deficit in verbal short-term memory. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 46(3), 304–316. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00352.x
Brown, G. D. A., Preece, T., & Hulme, C. (2000). Oscillator-based memory for serial order. Psychological Review, 107(1), 127–181. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.107.1.127
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106(3), 551–581. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.551
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (2006). A revised model of short-term memory and long-term learning of verbal sequences. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(4), 627–652. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.08.005
Campoy, G., Castellà, J., Provencio, V., Hitch, G. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2015). Automatic semantic encoding in verbal short-term memory: Evidence from the concreteness effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 68(4), 759–778. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.966248
Chekaf, M., Cowan, N., & Mathy, F. (2016). Chunk formation in immediate memory and how it relates to data compression. Cognition, 155, 96–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.024
Cousineau, D. (2012). Erratum to Cousineau (2005): Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 8(3), 182–182. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.08.3.p182
Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., & Gagnon, D. A. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychological Review, 104(4), 801–838. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.104.4.801
Dienes, Z. (2016). How Bayes factors change scientific practice. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 72, 78–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2015.10.003
Ding, N., Melloni, L., Zhang, H., Tian, X., & Poeppel, D. (2016). Cortical tracking of hierarchical linguistic structures in connected speech. Nature Neuroscience, 19(1), 158–164. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4186
Fallon, A. B., Mak, E., Tehan, G., & Daly, C. (2005). Lexicality and phonological similarity: A challenge for the retrieval-based account of serial recall? Memory, 13(3–4), 349–356. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210344000215
Farrell, S., & Lewandowsky, S. (2004). Modelling transposition latencies: Constraints for theories of serial order memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(1), 115–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.03.007
Freunberger, D., & Roehm, D. (2016). Semantic prediction in language comprehension: Evidence from brain potentials. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(9), 1193–1205. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2016.1205202
Gathercole, S. E., Frankish, C. R., Pickering, S. J., & Peaker, S. (1999). Phonotactic influences on short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(1), 84–95. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.25.3.562
Gorin, S., Kowialiewski, B., & Majerus, S. (2016). Domain-generality of timing-based serial order processes in short-term memory: New insights from musical and verbal domains. PLoS ONE, 11(12), e0168699. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0168699
Guérard, K., & Saint-Aubin, J. (2012). Assessing the effect of lexical variables in backward recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(2), 312–324. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025481
Guidali, G., Pisoni, A., Bolognini, N., & Papagno, C. (2019). Keeping order in the brain: The supramarginal gyrus and serial order in short-term memory. Cortex, 119, 89–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2019.04.009
Gupta, P., Lipinski, J., & Aktunc, E. (2005). Reexamining the phonological similarity effect in immediate serial recall: The roles of type of similarity, category cuing, and item recall. Memory & Cognition, 33(6), 1001–1016. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193208
Hartley, T., Hurlstone, M. J., & Hitch, G. J. (2016). Effects of rhythm on memory for spoken sequences: A model and tests of its stimulus-driven mechanism. Cognitive Psychology, 87, 135–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.05.001
Henson, R. N. A. (1998). Short-term memory for serial order: The startend model. Cognitive Psychology, 36(2), 73–137. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0685
Henson, R. N. A. (1999). Positional information in short-term memory: Relative or absolute? Memory & Cognition, 27(5), 915–927. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03198544
Henson, R. N. A., Hartley, T., Burgess, N., Hitch, G., & Flude, B. (2003). Selective interference with verbal short-term memory for serial order information: A new paradigm and tests of a timing-signal hypothesis. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56(8), 1307–1334. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980244000747
Hitch, G. J., Burgess, N., Towse, J. N., & Culpin, V. (1996). Temporal grouping effects in immediate recall: A working memory analysis. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 49(1), 116–139. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755609
Huang, L., & Awh, E. (2018). Chunking in working memory via content-free labels. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 23. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-18157-5
Hulme, C., Maughan, S., & Brown, G. D. A. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(6), 685–701. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90032-F
Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., Schweickert, R., Brown, G. D. A., Martin, S., & Stuart, G. (1997). Word-frequency effects on short-term memory tasks: evidence for a redintegration process in immediate serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(5), 1217–1232. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.23.5.1217
Hulme, C., Stuart, G., Brown, G. D. A., & Morin, C. (2003). High- and low-frequency words are recalled equally well in alternating lists: Evidence for associative effects in serial recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(4), 500–518. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00096-2
Hurlstone, M. J., & Hitch, G. J. (2015). How is the serial order of a spatial sequence represented? Insights from transposition latencies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(2), 295–324. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038223
Jefferies, E., Frankish, C. R., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). Lexical and semantic binding in verbal short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(1), 81–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.001
Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd éd.). Oxford University Press.
Kalm, K., & Norris, D. (2014). The representation of order information in auditory-verbal short-term memory. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 34(20), 6879–6886. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4104-13.2014
Kowialiewski, B., & Majerus, S. (2018). The non-strategic nature of linguistic long-term memory effects in verbal short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 101, 64–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.03.005
Kowialiewski, B., & Majerus, S. (2020). The varying nature of semantic effects in working memory. Cognition, 202, 104278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104278
Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 863. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863
Lee, M. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2014). Bayesian cognitive modeling: A practical course. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139087759
Lewandowsky, S., & Murdock, B. B. (1989). Memory for serial order. Psychological Review, 96(1), 25–57. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.1.25
Majerus, S. (2009). Verbal short-term memory and temporary activation of language representations: The importance of distinguishing item and order information. In A. S. Thorn & M. Page (Eds.), Interactions between short-term and long-term memory in the verbal domain (pp. 244–276). Psychology Press.
Majerus, S. (2013). Language repetition and short-term memory: An inte-grative framework. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 357. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00357
Majerus, S. (2019). Verbal working memory and the phonological buffer: The question of serial order. Cortex, 112, 122–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.016
Majerus, S., Attout, L., Artielle, M.-A., & Van der Kaa, M.-A. (2015). The heterogeneity of verbal short-term memory impairment in aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 77, 165–176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.08.010
Majerus, S., D'Argembeau, A., Martinez Perez, T., Belayachi, S., Van der Linden, M., Collette, F., Salmon, E., Seurinck, R., Fias, W., & Maquet, P. (2010). The commonality of neural networks for verbal and visual short-term memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(11), 2570–2593. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21378
Majerus, S., Martinez, T., & Oberauer, K. (2012). Two distinct origins of longterm learning effects in verbal short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(1), 38–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.07.006
Majerus, S., Van der Linden, M., Mulder, L., Meulemans, T., & Peters, F. (2004). Verbal short-term memory reflects the sublexical organization of the phonological language network: Evidence from an incidental pho-notactic learning paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(2), 297–306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.05.002
Martin, N., Minkina, I., Kohen, F. P., & Kalinyak-Fliszar, M. (2018). Assessment of linguistic and verbal short-term memory components of language abilities in aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 48, 199–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.02.006
Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., & Dell, G. S. (1996). Recovery in deep dysphasi: Evidence for a relation between auditory–verbal STM capacity and lexical errors in repetition. Brain and Language, 52(1), 83–113. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1996.0005
Martinez Perez, T., Poncelet, M., Salmon, E., & Majerus, S. (2015). Functional alterations in order short-term memory networks in adults with dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 40(7–8), 407–429. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2016.1153098
Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 314–324. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7
Mathy, F., Chekaf, M., & Cowan, N. (2018). Simple and complex working memory tasks allow similar benefits of information compression. Journal of Cognition, 1(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.31
Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4(2), 61–64. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.04.2.p061
Morey, R. D., & Rouder, J. N. (2014). BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs (Version 0.9.2+). https://richarddmorey.github.io/BayesFactor/
Papagno, C., Comi, A., Riva, M., Bizzi, A., Vernice, M., Casarotti, A., Fava, E., & Bello, L. (2017). Mapping the brain network of the phonological loop. Human Brain Mapping, 38(6), 3011–3024. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23569
Poirier, M., & Saint-Aubin, J. (1995). Memory for related and unrelated words: Further evidence on the influence of semantic factors in immediate serial recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48(2), 384–404. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749508401396
Poirier, M., Saint-Aubin, J., Mair, A., Tehan, G., & Tolan, A. (2015). Order recall in verbal short-term memory: The role of semantic networks. Memory & Cognition, 43(3), 489–499. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0470-6
R Development Core Team. (2008). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Romani, C., McAlpine, S., & Martin, R. C. (2008). Concreteness effects in different tasks: Implications for models of short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 61(2), 292–323. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210601147747
Roodenrys, S., Hulme, C., Lethbridge, A., Hinton, M., & Nimmo, L. M. (2002). Word-frequency and phonological-neighborhood effects on verbal short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(6), 1019–1034. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.6.1019
Saint-Aubin, J., Guérard, K., Chamberland, C., & Malenfant, A. (2014). Delineating the contribution of long-term associations to immediate recall. Memory, 22(4), 360–373. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.794242
Saint-Aubin, J., Ouellette, D., & Poirier, M. (2005). Semantic similarity and immediate serial recall: Is there an effect on all trials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(1), 171–177. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196364
Saint-Aubin, J., & Poirier, M. (1999a). The influence of long-term memory factors on immediate serial recall: An item and order
analysis. International Journal of Psychology, 34(5–6), 347–352. https://doi.org/10.1080/002075999399675
Saint-Aubin, J., & Poirier, M. (1999b). Semantic similarity and immediate serial recall: Is there a detrimental effect on order information? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 52(2), 367–394. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755814
Schönbrodt, F. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2018). Bayes factor design analysis: Planning for compelling evidence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 128–142. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1230-y
Schweickert, R. (1993). A multinomial processing tree model for degradation and redintegration in immediate recall. Memory & Cognition, 21(2), 168–175. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03202729
Schwering, S. C., & Macdonald, M. C. (2020). Verbal working memory as emergent from language comprehension and production. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14, 68. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00068
Tanida, Y., Ueno, T., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Saito, S. (2015). The roles of long-term phonotactic and lexical prosodic knowledge in phonological short-term memory. Memory & Cognition, 43(3), 500–519. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0482-2
Taylor, J. C., Macken, B., & Jones, D. M. (2015). A matter of emphasis: Linguistic stress habits modulate serial recall. Memory & Cognition, 43(3), 520–537. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0466-2
Tse, C.-S. (2009). The role of associative strength in the semantic related-ness effect on immediate serial recall. Memory, 17(8), 874–891. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210903376250
Tse, C.-S., & Altarriba, J. (2007). Testing the associative-link hypothesis in immediate serial recall: Evidence from word frequency and word imageability effects. Memory, 15(6), 675–690. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210701467186
Tse, C.-S., Li, Y., & Altarriba, J. (2011). The effect of semantic relatedness on immediate serial recall and serial recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 64(12), 2425–2437. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.604787
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Love, J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., Matzke, D., Morey, R. D. (2017). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1323-7
Walker, I., & Hulme, C. (1999). Concrete words are easier to recall than abstract words: Evidence for a semantic contribution to short-term serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(5), 1256–1271.
Yap, M. J., Lim, G. Y., & Pexman, P. M. (2015). Semantic richness effects in lexical decision: The role of feedback. Memory & Cognition, 43(8), 1148–1167. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0536-0