Brain; Consciousness; Neuroscience; philosophy; Disorders of Consciousness
Abstract :
[en] Science and philosophy still lack an overarching theory of consciousness. We suggest that a further step toward it requires going beyond the view of the brain as input-output machine and focusing on its intrinsic activity, which may express itself in two distinct modalities, i.e. aware and unaware. We specifically investigate the predisposition of the brain to evaluate and to model the world. These intrinsic activities of the brain retain a deep relation with consciousness. In fact the ability of the brain to evaluate and model the world can develop in two modalities, implicit or explicit, that correspond to what we usually refer to as the unconscious and consciousness, and both are multilevel configurations of the brain along a continuous and dynamic line. Starting from an empirical understanding of the brain as intrinsically active and plastic, we here distinguish between higher cognitive functions and basic phenomenal consciousness, suggesting that the latter might characterize the brain’s intrinsic activity as such, even if at a very basic level. We proceed to explore possible impacts of the notion of intrinsic cerebral phenomenality on our understanding of consciousness and its disorders, particularly on the diagnosis and management of patients with disorders of consciousness.
Disciplines :
Philosophy & ethics Neurology
Author, co-author :
Farisco, Michele; Centre for Research Ethics and Bioethics, University of Uppsala, Sweden
Laureys, Steven ; Université de Liège - ULiège > GIGA Consciousness: Coma Science Group
Evers, Katinka; Centre for Research Ethics and Bioethics, University of Uppsala, Sweden
Language :
English
Title :
The intrinsic activity of the brain and its relation to levels and disorders of consciousness
Publication date :
2017
Journal title :
Mind and Matter
ISSN :
1611-8812
eISSN :
2051-3003
Publisher :
Imprint Academic, United Kingdom
Volume :
15
Issue :
2
Pages :
197-219
Peer reviewed :
Peer reviewed
European Projects :
H2020 - 720270 - HBP SGA1 - Human Brain Project Specific Grant Agreement 1
Funders :
CRB, Citrus Research Board CE - Commission Européenne
Andrews-Hanna J.R. (2012): The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. Neuroscientist 18(3), 251-270.
Andrews-Hanna J.R., Smallwood J., and Spreng R.N. (2014): The default network and self-generated thought: component process, dynamic contreoll and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 1316, 29-52.
Aru J., Bachmann T., Singer W., and Melloni M. (2012): Distilling the neural correlates of consciousness. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Review 36(2), 737-746.
Baars B.J. and Laureys S. (2005): One, not two, neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Science 9(6), 269; author reply 270.
Barttfeld P., Uhrig L., Sitt J. D., Sigman M., Jarraya B., and Dehaene S. (2015): Signature of consciousness in the dynamics of resting-state brain activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 112, 887-892.
Bayne T., Hohwy J., and Owen A.M. (2016): Are there levels of consciousness? Trends in Cognitive Science 20(6), 405-413.
Berlin H.A. (2011): The neural basis of the dynamic unconscious. Neuropsy-choanalysis 13(1), 5-31.
Block N. (1995): On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18(2), 227-287.
Boly M., Balteau E., Schnakers C., Degueldre C., Moonen G., Luxen A., Phillips C., Peigneux P., Maquet P., and Laureys S. (2007a): Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict somatosensory perception in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 104, 12187-12192.
Boly M., Coleman M.R., Davis M.H., Hampshire A., Bor D., Moonen G., Ma-quet P.A., Pickard J.D., Laureys S., and Owen A.M. (2007b): When thoughts become action: An fMRI paradigm to study volitional brain activity in non-communicative brain injured patients. Neuroimage 36(3), 979-992.
Boly M., Phillips C., Tshibanda L., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Schabus M., Dang-Vu T.T., Moonen G., Hustinx R., Maquet P., and Laureys S. (2008): Intrinsic brain activity in altered states of consciousness: how conscious is the default mode of brain function? Annals of the New York Academy of Science 1129, 119-129.
Boly M., Tshibanda L., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Noirhomme Q., Schnakers C., Ledoux D., Boveroux P., Garweg C., Lambermont B., Phillips C., Luxen A., Moonen G., Bassetti C., Maquet P., and Laureys S. (2009): Functional connectivity in the default network during resting state is preserved in a vegetative but not in a brain dead patient. Human Brain Mapping 30(8), 2393-2400.
Bruno M.A., Majerus S., Boly M., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Schnakers C., Gosseries O., Boveroux P., Kirsch M., Demertzi A., Bernard C., Hustinx R., Moonen G., and Laureys S. (2012): Functional neuroanatomy underlying the clinical subcategorization of minimally conscious state patients. Journal of Neurology 259(6), 1087-1098.
Cerullo M.A. (2015): The problem with phi: a critic of integrated information theory. Plos Computational Biology 11(9), e1004286.
Chalmers D. (2000): What is a neural correlate of consciousness? In Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Empirical and Conceptual Questions, ed. by T. Metzinger, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 17-39.
Changeux J.-P. (1986): Neuronal Man: the Biology ofMind, Oxford University Press, New York.
Changeux J.-P. (2004): The Physiology of Truth: Neuroscience and Human Knowledge, Havard University Press, Cambrige.
Changeux J.P., Courrege, P., and Danchin A. (1973): A theory of the epigenesis of neuronal networks by selective stabilization of synapses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 70, 2974-2978.
Crick F. and Koch K. (1990): Towards a neurobiological theory ofconsciousness. Seminars in Neuroscience 2, 263-275.
Dehaene S. (2014): Consciousness and the Brain: Deciphering How the Brain Codes Our Thoughts, Viking Adult, New York.
Dehaene S. and Changeux J.P. (2005): Ongoing spontaneous activity controls access to consciousness: A neuronal model for inattentional blindness. PLos Biology 3(5), e 141.
Dehaene S. and Changeux J.P. (2011): Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron 70(2), 200-227.
Dehaene S., Changeux J.P., Naccache L., Sackur J., and Sergent C. (2006): Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Science 10(5), 204-211.
Dijksterhuis A. and Nordgren L.F. (2006): A theory of unconscious thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science 1(2), 95-109.
Dudai Y. and Evers K. (2014): To simulate or not to simulate: What are the questions? Neuron 84(2), 254-261.
Edelman G.M. (1987): Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection, Basic Books, New York.
Edelman G.M. (1992): Bright Air, Brilliant Fire: On the Matter of the Mind, Basic Books, New York.
Evers K. (2009): Neuroetique. Quand la matiere s’eveille, Odile Jacob, Paris.
Evers K. and Sigman M. (2013): Possibilities and limits of mind-reading: A neurophilosophical perspective. Consciousness and Cognition 22, 887-897.
Farisco M. and Evers K. (2017): The ethical relevance of the unconscious. Submitted to Philosophy, Ethics and Humanities in Medicine.
Fisher D.B. and Truog R.D. (2017): The problems with fixating on consciousness in disorders of consciousness. AJOB Neuroscience 8(3), 135-140.
Friston K. (2010): The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11(2), 127-138.
Frith C. (2007): Making up the Mind: How the Brain Creates Our Mental World, Wiley-Blackwell, London.
Giacino J.T., Fins J.J., Laureys S. and Schiff N.D. (2014): Disorders of consciousness after acquired brain injury: The state of the science. Nature Reviews Neurology 10(2), 99-14.
Gosseries O., Vanhaudenhuyse A, Bruno MA, Demertzi A, Schnakers C, Boly M.M., Maudoux A, Moonen G., and Laureys S. (2011): Disorders of consciousness: Coma, vegetative and minimally conscious states. In Experimental Insights into Meditation, Waking, Sleep and Dreams, ed. by D. Cvetkovic and I. Cosic, Springer, Heidelberg, pp. 29-55.
Greicius M.D., Kiviniemi V., Tervonen O., Vainionpaa V., Alahuhta S., Reiss A. L., and Menon V. (2008): Persistent default-mode network connectivity during light sedation. Human Brain Mapping 29(7), 839-847.
Gusnard D.A. and Raichle M.E. (2001): Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2(10), 685-694.
Hassin R.R. (2013): Yes it can: On the functional abilities of the human unconscious. Perspectives on Psychological Science 8(2), 195-207.
Hassin R.R., Uleman J.S., and Bargh J.A. (2005): The New Unconscious, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Hatzimoysis A. (2007): The case against unconscious emotions. Analysis 67(4), 292-299.
Heine L., Soddu A., Gomez F., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Tshibanda L., Thonnard M., Charland-Verville V., Kirsch M., Laureys S., and Demertzi A. (2012): Resting state networks and consciousness: Alterations of multiple resting state network connectivity in physiological, pharmacological, and pathological consciousness states. Frontiers of Psychology 3, 295.
Hesselmann G. and Moors P. (2015): Definitely maybe: Can unconscious processes perform the same functions as conscious processes? Frontiers ofPsychol-ogy 6, 584.
Hohwy J. and Fox E. (2012): Preserved aspects of consciousness in disorders of consciousness: A review and conceptual analysis. Journal of Consciousness Studies 19(3-4), 87-120.
Horovitz S.G., Braun A.R., Carr W.S., Picchioni D., Balkin T.J., Fukunaga M., and Duyn J.H. (2009): Decoupling of the brain’s default mode network during deep sleep. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 106, 11376-11381.
Huang Z., Dai X., Wu X., Yang Z., Liu D., Hu J., Gao L., Tang W., Mao Y., Jin Y., Wu X., Liu B., Zhang Y, Lu L., Laureys S., Weng X., and Northoff G. (2014): The self and its resting state in consciousness: An investigation of the vegetative state. Human Brain Mapping 35(5), 1997-2008.
Huang Z., Zhang J., Longtin A., Dumont G., Duncan N. W., Pokorny J., Qin P., Dai R., Ferri F., Weng X, and and Northoff G. (2015): Is there a nonadditive interaction between spontaneous and evoked activity? Phase-dependence and its relation to the temporal structure of scale-free brain activity. Cerebral Cortex 27(2), 1037-1059.
Huang Z., Zhang J., Wu J., Qin P.,, Wu X.,, Wang Z.,, Dai R., Li Y., Liang W., Mao Y., Yang Z., Zhang J., Wolff A., and Northoff G. (2016): Decoupled temporal variability and signal synchronization of spontaneous brain activity in loss of consciousness: An fMRI study in anesthesia. Neuroimage A 124, 693-703.
Kastrup B. (2017): There is an “unconscious”, but it may well be conscious. Europe s Journal ofPsychology 13(3), 559-572.
Koch C., Massimini M., Boly M., and Tononi G. (2016): Neural correlates of consciousness: Progress and problems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 17(5), 307-321.
Koch C. and Tsuchiya N. (2012): Attention and consciousness: Related yet different. Trends in Cognitive Science 16(2), 103-105.
Kornhuber H.H. and Deecke L. (1965): Hirnpotentialanderungen bei Will-kurbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspoten-tial und reafferente Potentiale. Pfliigers Archiv fur die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere 284(1), 1-17.
Kouider S., de Gardelle V., Dehaene S., Dupoux E., and Pallier C. (2010a): Cerebral bases of subliminal speech priming. Neuroimage 49(1), 922-929.
Kouider S., de Gardelle V., Sackur J., and Dupoux E. (2010b): How rich is consciousness? The partial awareness hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Science 14(7), 301-307.
Lamme V.A. (2006): Towards a true neural stance on consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Science 10(11), 494-501.
Laureys S. (2005): The neural correlate of (un)awareness: Lessons from the vegetative state. Trends in Cognitive Science (12), 556-559.
Laureys S. (2015): Un Si Brillant Cerveau. Les etats limites de consciences, Odile Jacob, Paris.
Laureys S., Owen A.M., and Schiff N. D. (2004): Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. The Lancet Neurology 3(9), 537-546.
LeDoux J.E. (2002): Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are, Viking, New York.
Merker B. (2007): Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neu-roscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Science 30(1), 63-81; discussion pp. 81-134.
Metzinger T., ed. (2000): Neural Correlates of Consciousness. Empirical and Conceptual Questions, MIT Press, Cambridge.
Monti M.M., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Coleman M.R., Boly M., Pickard J.D., Tshi-banda L., Owen A.M., and Laureys S. (2010): Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. New England Journal ofMedicine 362(7), 579-589.
Mudrik L., Faivre N., and Koch C. (2014): Information integration without awareness. Trends in Cognitive Science 18(9), 488-496.
Nagel T. (1974): What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review 8(4), 435-450.
Nir Y., Staba R.J., Andrillon T., Vyazovskiy V.V., Cirelli C., Fried I., and Tononi G. (2011): Regional slow waves and spindles in human sleep. Neuron 70(1), 153-169.
Noirhomme Q., Soddu A., Lehembre R., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Boveroux, P., Boly M., and Laureys S. (2010): Brain connectivity in pathological and pharmacological coma. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 4, 160.
Northoff G. (2013a): Unlocking the Brain Volume 1, Coding, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Northoff G. (2013b): What the brain’s intrinsic activity can tell us about consciousness? A tri-dimensional view. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37(4), 726-738.
Northoff G. (2014a): Minding the Brain: A Guide to Philosophy and Neuro-science, Palgrave Macmillan, Basingstoke.
Northoff G. (2014b): Unlocking the Brain Volume 2, Consciousness, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Northoff G. (2016): Neuroscience and Whitehead I: Neuro-Ecological Model of Brain. Axiomathes 26(3), 219-252.
Ohman A., Flykt A., and Lundqvist D. (2000): Unconscious emotion: Evolutionary perspectives, psychophysiological data and neuropsychological mechanisms. In Cognitive Neuroscience of Emotion, ed. by L. Nadel, G. Ahern and R.D. Lane, Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 296-327.
Overgaard M. (2017): The status and future of consicousness research. Frontiers in Psychology 8, 1719.
Parvizi J. and Damasio A. (2001): Consciousness and the brainstem. Cognition 79(1-2), 135-160.
Pereira A. and Lehmann D., eds. (2015): The Unity ofMind, Brain and World. Current Perspectives on a Science of Consciousness, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Prinz J. (2015): Unconscious perception. In Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Perception, ed. by M. Matthen, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 371-392.
Raichle M.E. (2015): The restless brain: How intrinsic activity organizes brain function. Philosophical Transaction of the Royal Society of London Series B 370(1668).
Raichle M.E., MacLeod A.M., Snyder A.Z., Powers W.J., Gusnard D.A. and Shulman G.L. (2001): A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 98, 676-682.
Raichle M.E. and Snyder A.Z. (2007): A default mode of brain function: A brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage 37(4), 1083-1090; discussion pp. 1097-1089.
Rosanova M., Gosseries O., Casarotto S., Boly M., Casali A.G., Bruno M.A., Mariotti M., Boveroux P., Tononi G., Laureys S., and Massimini M. (2012): Recovery of cortical effective connectivity and recovery of consciousness in vegetative patients. Brain 135(4), 1308-1320.
Rosazza C., Andronache A., Sattin D., Bruzzone M.G., Marotta G., Nigri A., Ferraro S., Sebastiano D.R., Porcu L., Bersano A., Benti R., Leonardi M., D’Incerti L., Minati L., and Coma I. (2016): Multimodal study of default-mode network integrity in disorders of consciousness. Annals of Neurology 79, 841-853.
Sanders R.D., Tononi G., Laureys S., and Sleigh J.W. (2012): Unresponsiveness not equal unconsciousness. Anesthesiology 116, 946-959.
Schier E. (2009): Identifying phenomenal consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 18(1), 216-222.
Sklar A.Y., Levy N., Goldstein A., Mand R., Maril A., and Hassin R.R. (2012): Reading and doing arithmetic nonconsciously. Proceedings of the National Academy ofSciences ofthe USA 109, 19614-19619.
Smith R. and Lane R.D. (2016): Unconscious emotion: A cognitive neuroscien-tific perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 69, 216-238.
van Boxtel J.J., Tsuchiya N., and Koch C. (2010): Consciousness and attention: On sufficiency and necessity. Frontiers in Psychology 1, 217.
Vanhaudenhuyse A., Demertzi A., Schabus M., Noirhomme Q., Bredart S., Boly M., Phillips C., Soddu A., Luxen A., Moonen G. and Laureys S. (2011): Two distinct neuronal networks mediate the awareness of environment and of self. Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 23(3), 570-578.
Vanhaudenhuyse A., Noirhomme Q., Tshibanda J., Bruno M.A., Boveroux P., Schnakers C., Soddu A.,perlbag V., Ledoux D., Brichant J.F., Moonen G., Maquet P., Greicius M.D., Laureys S., and Boly M. (2010): Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-communicative braindamaged patients. Brain 133(1), 161-171.
Whitfield-Gabrieli S., Moran J.M., Nieto-Castanon A., Triantafyllou C., Saxe R. and Gabrieli J.D. (2011): Associations and dissociations between default and self-reference networks in the human brain. Neuroimage 55(1), 225-232.
Winkielman P. and Berridge K.C. (2004): Unconscious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science 13(3), 120-123.