[en] From an early age, children develop stereotypes toward older adults leading to potential ageism. In young adults, ageism includes conversational changes, also known as elderspeak, characterized by louder, slower, and simplified speech. Although it has direct consequence on older adults, to date no studies have explored elderspeak in children. We invited 137 Belgian children aged 7 to 12 to take part in a guessing game through a dissimulated Skype session. The child had to make two female interlocutors in turn, one young (25 years old) and the other old (75), guess two different words each. During the session, children remained unaware of the real purpose of the game. Prosody, verbal fluency, and semantic content of their speech were measured using the Praat and VocabProfil software. The results, analyzed using mixed-design ANOVA, showed that children spoke louder to the older interlocutor and tended to judge her less competent to guess words than the younger participant. When the older person was second in turn, children engaged in lengthier and more detailed explanations. Unexpectedly, positive views on older people correlated with higher voice intensity, suggesting the child’s differential verbal attitudes may translate a form of benevolent ageism. In conclusion, significant speech accommodation can be detected in 7-12 year-old children when they speak to an older compared to a younger interlocutor. This accommodation is characterized by louder voice and lengthier explanations.
Disciplines :
Treatment & clinical psychology
Author, co-author :
Flamion, Allison ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Psychologie > Département de Psychologie
Missotten, Pierre ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Psychologie > Psychologie de la sénescence et du vieillissement
Goffinet, Alysson; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Psychologie > Psychologie de la sénescence et du vieillissement
Kukor, Léna; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Psychologie > Psychologie de la sénescence et du vieillissement
Nagy, Noemi; University of Bern > Department of Work and Organizational Psychology
Adam, Stéphane ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Psychologie > Psychologie de la sénescence et du vieillissement
Language :
English
Title :
Speech accommodation toward older people in 7- to 12-year-old children
Álvarez-Cañizo, M., Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2015). The role of reading fluency in children’s text comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1810. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01810
Babcock, R. E., MaloneBeach, E. E., Hannighofer, J., & Woodworth-Hou, B. (2016). Development of a children’s IAT to measure bias against the elderly. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 14, 167–178. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2016.1195245
Balázs, J. (2013). Patterns of ageism in different age groups. Journal of European Psychology Students, 4, 16–36. https://doi.org/10.5334/jeps.aw
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2017). Praat: Doing phoneHcs by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.0.29, http://www.praat.org/.
Butler, R. N. (1969). Age-ism, another form of bigotry. The Gerontologist, 9, 243–246. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/9.4_Part_1.243
Caporael, L. R. (1981). The paralanguage of caregiving: Baby talk to the institutionalized aged. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 876–884. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.40.5.876
Cary, L. A., Chasteen, A. L., & Remedios, J. (2017). The ambivalent ageism scale: Developing and validating a scale to measure benevolent and hostile ageism. The Gerontologist, 57, 27–36. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnw118
Davidson, D., Luo, Z., & Fulton, B. R. (2008). Stereotyped views of older adults in children from the people’s Republic of China and from the United States. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 5, 7–24. https://doi.org/10.1300/J194v05n04_02
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149–1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
Flamion, A., Missotten, P., Marquet, M., & Adam, S. (2019). Impact of contact with grandparents on children’s and adolescents’ views on the elderly. Child Development, 90, 1155–1169. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12992
Giles, H., Coupland, N., & Coupland, J. (1991). Accommodation theory: Communication, context, and consequence. In H. Giles, N. Coupland & J. Coupland (Eds.), Contexts of accommodation: Developments in applied sociolinguistics (pp. 1–68). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Grimme, T. M., Buchanan, J., & Afflerbach, S. (2015). Understanding elderspeak from the perspective of certified nursing assistants. Journal of Gerontological Nursing, 41, 42–49. https://doi.org/10.3928/00989134-20151015-05
Isaacs, L. W., & Bearison, D. J. (1986). The development of children’s prejudice against the aged. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 23, 175–194. https://doi.org/10.2190/8GVR-XJQY-LFTH-E0A1
Kemper, S. (1994). Elderspeak: Speech accomodations to older adults. Aging and Cognition, 1, 17–28. https://doi.org/10.1080/09289919408251447
Kemper, S., & Harden, T. (1999). Experimentally disentangling what’s beneficial about elderspeak from what’s not. Psychology and Aging, 14, 656–670. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.14.4.656
Kemper, S., Othick, M., Gerhing, H., Gubarchuk, J., & Bellington, C. (1998). The effects of practicing speech accommodations to older adults. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 175–192. https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640001002X
Kemper, S., Vandeputte, D., Rice, K., Cheung, H., & Gubarchuk, J. (1995). Speech adjustments to aging during a referential communication task. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 14, 40–59. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X95141003
Kwong See, S. T., & Nicoladis, E. (2010). Impact of contact on the development of children's positive stereotyping about aging language competence. Educational Gerontology, 36, 52–66. https://doi.org/10.1080/03601270903018352
Kwong See, S. T., Rasmussen, C., & Pertman, S. Q. (2012). Measuring children’s age stereotyping using a modified Piagetian conservation task. Educational Gerontology, 38, 149–165. https://doi.org/10.1080/03601277.2010.515891
Levy, B. R., & Langer, E. (1994). Aging free from negative stereotypes: Successful memory in China among the American deaf. Journal of Psychology and Social Psychology, 66, 989–997. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.6.989
Luo, B., Zhou, K., Jin, E. J., Newman, A., & Liang, J. (2013). Ageism among college students: A comparative study between U.S. and China. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 28, 49–63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10823-013-9186-5
Miller, S. M., Blalock, J., & Ginsburg, H. J. (1984). Children and the aged: Attitudes, contact, and discriminative ability. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 19, 47–53. https://doi.org/10.2190/HRA5-PJJR-KKLR-D3PC
Montepare, J. M., Steinberg, J., & Rosenberg, B. (1992). Characteristics of vocal communication between young adults and their parents and grandparents. Communication Research, 19, 479–492. https://doi.org/10.1177/009365092019004005
Randler, C., Vollmer, C., Wilhelm, D., Flessner, M., & Hummel, E. (2014). Attitudes towards the elderly among German adolescents. Educational Gerontology, 40, 230–238. https://doi.org/10.1080/03601277.2013.802187
Ryan, E. B., Giles, H., Bartolucci, G., & Henwood, K. (1986). Psycholinguistic and social psychological components of communication by and with the elderly. Language and Communication, 6, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/0271-5309(86)90002-9
Samuelsson, C., Adolfsson, E., & Persson, H. (2013). The use and characteristics of elderspeak in Swedish geriatric institutions. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 27, 616–631. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2013.773382
Schoemann, A. M., & Branscombe, N. R. (2011). Looking young for your age: Perceptions of anti-aging action. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 86–95. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.738
Schroyen, S., Adam, S., Marquet, M., Jerusalem, G., Thiel, S., Giraudet, A.-L., & Missotten, P. (2017). Communication of healthcare professionnals: Is there ageism? European Journal of Cancer Care, 27, e12780. https://doi.org/10.1111/ecc.12780
Selva, T., Jones, G., & Goodfellow, R. (2010). French lists for Vocabprofile. https://lextutor.ca/vp/fr/
Tajfel, H. (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of group. London: Academic Press.
Thompson, E. H., Weaver, A. J. (2016). Making connections: The legacy of an intergenerational program. The Gerontologist, 56, 909–918. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnv064
Vauclair, C.-M., Borges Rodrigues, R., Marques, S., Esteves, C. S., Cunha, F., & Gerardo, F. (2018). Doddering but dear… even in the eyes of young children? Age stereotyping and prejudice in childhood and adolescence. International Journal of Psychology, 53, 63–70. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12430
Wandke, H., Sengpiel, M., & Sönksen, M. (2012). Myths about older people’s use of information and communication technology. Gerontology, 58, 564–570. https://doi.org/10.1159/000339104
Williams, A., & Garrett, P. (2012). Teenagers’ perceptions of communication and ‘good communication’ with peers, young adults, and older adults. Language Awareness, 21, 267–278. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658416.2011.604422
Williams, K., Kemper, S., & Hummert, M. L. (2003). Improving nursing home communication: An intervention to reduce elderspeak. The Gerontologist, 43, 242–247. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/43.2.242
Williams, K., Kemper, S., & Hummert, M. L. (2004). Enhancing communication with older adults: Overcoming elderspeak. Journal of Gerontological Nursing, 30, 17–25. https://doi.org/10.3928/0098-9134-20041001-08