Albouy, P., Peretz, I., Bermudez, P., Zatorre, R. J., Tillmann, B., & Caclin, A. (2018). Specialized neural dynamics for verbal and tonal memory: fMRI evidence in congenital amusia. Human Brain Mapping, 1-13. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1002/hbm.24416
Attout, L., Van der Kaa, M.-A., George, M., & Majerus, S. (2012). Dissociating short-term memory and language impairment: The importance of item and serial order information. Aphasiology, 26(3-4), 355-382. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/0268 7038.2011.604303
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. DOI: https: //do i.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1
Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., & Camos, V. (2004). Time constraints and resource sharing in adults' working memory spans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 83-100. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.83
Berkowska, M., & Dalla Bella, S. (2013). Uncovering phenotypes of poor-pitch singing: the Sung Performance Battery (SPB). Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 714. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg. 2013. 00714
Berz, W. L. (1995). Working Memory in Music: A Theoretical Model. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12(3), 353-364. DOI: https://doi. org/10.2307/40286188
Brock, J., & Jarrold, C. (2005). Serial order reconstruction in Down syndrome: evidence for a selective deficit in verbal short-term memory. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(3), 304-316. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610. 2004.00352.x
Brown, G. D. A., Preece, T., & Hulme, C. (2000). Oscillator-based memory for serial order. Psychological Review, 107(1), 127-181. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.127
Buchsbaum, B. R., & D'Esposito, M. (2008). The Search for the Phonological Store: From Loop to Convolution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(5), 762-778. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/ jocn.2008.20501
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1992). Toward a network model of the articulatory loop. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(4), 429-460. DOI: https://doi.org/10. 1016/0749-596X(92)90022-P
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (2006). A revised model of short-term memory and long-term learning of verbal sequences. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(4), 627-652. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jml.2006.08.005
Content, A., Mousty, P., & Radeau, M. (1990). BRULEX. Une base de données lexicales informatisée pour le français écrit et parlé. Année Psychologique, 90, 551-566. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3406/ psy.1990.29428
Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof: oso/9780195119107.001.0001
Deutsch, D. (1970). Tones and Numbers: Specificity of Interference in Immediate Memory. Science, 168(3939), 1604-1605. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.168.3939.1604
Farrell, S., & Lewandowsky, S. (2002). An endogenous distributed model of ordering in serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(1), 59-79. DOI: https://doi. org/10.3758/BF03196257
Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory & Cognition, 23(1), 83-94. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/ BF03210559
Gathercole, S. E., Frankish, C. R., Pickering, S. J., & Peaker, S. M. (1999). Phonotactic influences on short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(1), 84-95. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393. 25.1.84
Gorin, S., Kowialiewski, B., & Majerus, S. (2016). Domain-Generality of Timing-Based Serial Order Processes in Short-Term Memory: New Insights from Musical and Verbal Domains. PLOS ONE, 11(12), e0168699. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0168699
Gorin, S., Mengal, P., & Majerus, S. (2018a). A comparison of serial order short-term memory effects across verbal and musical domains. Memory & Cognition, 46(3), 464-481. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/ s13421-017-0778-0
Greenspon, E. B., Pfordresher, P. Q., & Halpern, A. R. (2017). Pitch Imitation Ability in Mental Transformations of Melodies. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 34(5), 585-604. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1525/mp.2017.34.5.585
Gupta, P. (2003). Examining the relationship between word learning, nonword repetition, and immediate serial recall in adults. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 56(7), 1213-1236. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1080/02724980343000071
Hartley, T., Hurlstone, M. J., & Hitch, G. J. (2016). Effects of rhythm on memory for spoken sequences: A model and tests of its stimulus-driven mechanism. Cognitive Psychology, 87, 135-178. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cogpsych.2016.05.001
Henson, R. N. A. (1996). Short-term memory for serial order. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Cambridge University, UK. Retrieved from http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/ people/rik.henson/personal/thesis/
Henson, R. N. A. (1998). Short-Term Memory for Serial Order: The Start-End Model. Cognitive Psychology, 36(2), 73-137. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0685
Henson, R. N. A., Hartley, T., Burgess, N., Hitch, G. J., & Flude, B. (2003). Selective interference with verbal short-term memory for serial order information: A new paradigm and tests of a timing-signal hypothesis. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 56(8), 1307-1334. DOI: https://doi.org/10. 1080/02724980244000747
Hickok, G., Buchsbaum, B. R., Humphries, C., & Muftuler, T. (2003). Auditory-Motor Interaction Revealed by fMRI: Speech, Music, and Working Memory in Area Spt. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(5), 673-682. DOI: https://doi.org/10. 1162/089892903322307393
Hughes, R. W., Chamberland, C., Tremblay, S., & Jones, D. M. (2016). Perceptual-motor determinants of auditory-verbal serial short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 90, 126-146. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jml.2016.04.006
Hulme, C., Maughan, S., & Brown, G. D. A. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(6), 685-701. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90032-F
Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., Schweickert, R., Brown, G. D. A., Martin, S., & Stuart, G. (1997). Word-frequency effects on short-term memory tasks: Evidence for a redintegration process in immediate serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(5), 1217-1232. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1037/0278-7393.23.5.1217
Hurlstone, M. J., Hitch, G. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2014). Memory for serial order across domains: An overview of the literature and directions for future research. Psychological Bulletin, 140(2), 339-373. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/ a0034221
Hutchins, S., Larrouy-Maestri, P., & Peretz, I. (2014). Singing ability is rooted in vocal-motor control of pitch. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(8), 2522-2530. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/ s13414-014-0732-1
Hutchins, S., Zarate, J. M., Zatorre, R. J., & Peretz, I. (2010). An acoustical study of vocal pitch matching in congenital amusia. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 127(1), 504-512. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3270391
Janata, P., Tillmann, B., & Bharucha, J. J. (2002). Listening to polyphonic music recruits domain-general attention and working memory circuits. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(2), 121-140. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/ CABN.2.2.121
JASP Team. (2018). JASP (version 0.9)[Computer software]. Retrieved from https:// jasp-stats.org/
Jefferies, E., Jones, R. W., Bateman, D., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2005). A semantic contribution to nonword recall? Evidence for intact phonological processes in semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(2), 183-212. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1080/02643290442000068
Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Cognitive Foundations of Musical Pitch. New-York, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof: oso/9780195148367.001.0001
Krumhansl, C. L., & Kessler, E. J. (1982). Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys. Psychological Review, 89(4), 334-368. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1037/0033-295X.89.4.334
Larrouy-Maestri, P., & Morsomme, D. (2014). Criteria and tools for objectively analysing the vocal accuracy of a popular song. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 39(1), 11-18. DOI: https://doi.org/10.31 09/14015439.2012.696139
Leclercq, A.-L., & Majerus, S. (2010). Serial-order short-term memory predicts vocabulary development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 417-427. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1037/a0018540
Lee, C. L., & Estes, W. K. (1981). Item and order information in short-term memory: Evidence for multilevel perturbation processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 7(3), 149-169. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.7.3.149
Lewandowsky, S., & Farrell, S. (2008). Short-Term Memory: New Data and a Model. In H. R. Brian (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 49, pp. 1-48). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)00001-7
Lewandowsky, S., & Murdock, B. B. (1989). Memory for serial order. Psychological Review, 96(1), 25-57. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.1.25
Majerus, S. (2009). Verbal short-term memory and temporary activation of language representations: The importance of distinguishing item and order information. In A. S. C. Thorn, & M. P. A. Page (Eds.), Interactions between short-term and long-term memory in the verbal domain (pp. 244-276). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Majerus, S. (2013). Language repetition and short-term memory: an integrative framework. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 357. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/ fnhum.2013.00357
Majerus, S. (2018). Verbal working memory and the phonological buffer: The question of serial order. Cortex. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.016
Majerus, S., Martinez Perez, T., & Oberauer, K. (2012). Two distinct origins of long-term learning effects in verbal short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(1), 38-51. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.07.006
Majerus, S., Metz-Lutz, M.-N., Van der Kaa, M.-A., Van der Linden, M., & Poncelet, M. (2007). Evidence for a further fractionation of the verbal STM system: Selective impairments for item and serial order retention capacities in STM patients. Brain and Language, 103(1-2), 185-186. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.07.107
Majerus, S., Norris, D. G., & Patterson, K. (2007). What does a patient with semantic dementia remember in verbal short-term memory? Order and sound but not words. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(2), 131-151. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1080/02643290600989376
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Greffe, C., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Relations between vocabulary development and verbal short-term memory: The relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and item information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93(2), 95-119. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jecp.2005.07.005
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Van der Linden, M., & Weekes, B. S. (2008). Lexical learning in bilingual adults: The relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and phonological knowledge. Cognition, 107(2), 395-419. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.10.003
Majerus, S., & Van der Linden, M. (2003). Long-term memory effects on verbal short-term memory: A replication study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(2), 303-310. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1348/026151003765264101
Majerus, S., Van der Linden, M., Mulder, L., Meulemans, T., & Peters, F. (2004). Verbal short-term memory reflects the sublexical organization of the phonological language network: Evidence from an incidental phonotactic learning paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(2), 297-306. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.05.002
Mathias, B., Pfordresher, P. Q., & Palmer, C. (2015). Context and meter enhance long-range planning in music performance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1040. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/ fnhum.2014.01040
Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 314-324. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7
Morey, R. D., & Rouder, J. N. (2015). BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs. Retrieved from http:// cran.r-project.org/package=BayesFactor
Nees, M. A., Corrini, E., Leong, P., & Harris, J. (2017). Maintenance of memory for melodies: Articulation or attentional refreshing? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(6), 1964-1970. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1269-9
Oberauer, K., & Hein, L. (2012). Attention to Information in Working Memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(3), 164-169. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1177/0963721412444727
Oberauer, K., Lewandowsky, S., Farrell, S., Jarrold, C., & Greaves, M. (2012). Modeling working memory: An interference model of complex span. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(5), 779-819. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0272-4
Ockelford, A. (2007). A Music Module in Working Memory? Evidence from the Performance of a Prodigious Musical Savant. Musicae Scientiae, 11(2 suppl), 5-36. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1177/10298649070110S202
Page, M. P. A., & Norris, D. G. (1998). The primacy model: A new model of immediate serial recall. Psychological Review, 105(4), 761-781. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.4.761-781
Patel, A. D. (2012). Language, music, and the brain: a resource-sharing framework. In P. Rebuschat, M. Rohrmeier, J. Hawkins, & I. Cross (Eds.), Language and Music as Cognitive Systems (pp. 204-223). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof: oso/9780199553426.003.0022
Pechmann, T., & Mohr, G. (1992). Interference in memory for tonal pitch: Implications for a working-memory model. Memory & Cognition, 20(3), 314-320. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/ BF03199668
Peretz, I., Champod, A. S., & Hyde, K. L. (2003). Varieties of Musical Disorders: The Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999(1), 58-75. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1196/annals.1284.006
Pfordresher, P. Q., & Brown, S. (2007). Poor- Pitch Singing in the Absence of "Tone Deafness." Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25(2), 95-115. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2007.25.2.95
Pfordresher, P. Q., & Brown, S. (2009). Enhanced production and perception of musical pitch in tone language speakers. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(6), 1385-1398. DOI: https://doi. org/10.3758/APP.71.6.1385
Pfordresher, P. Q., & Halpern, A. R. (2013). Auditory imagery and the poor-pitch singer. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(4), 747-753. DOI: https://doi. org/10.3758/s13423-013-0401-8
Pfordresher, P. Q., Halpern, A. R., & Greenspon, E. B. (2015). A Mechanism for Sensorimotor Translation in Singing. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 32(3), 242-253. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1525/mp.2015.32.3.242
Pfordresher, P. Q., & Mantell, J. T. (2014). Singing with yourself: Evidence for an inverse modeling account of poor-pitch singing. Cognitive Psychology, 70(0), 31-57. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cogpsych.2013.12.005
Pfordresher, P. Q., Palmer, C., & Jungers, M. K. (2007). Speed, Accuracy, and Serial Order in Sequence Production. Cognitive Science, 31(1), 63-98. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1080/03640210709336985
Plancher, G., Lévêque, Y., Fanuel, L., Piquandet, G., & Tillmann, B. (2018). Boosting maintenance in working memory with temporal regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(5), 812-818. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000481
Poirier, M., & Saint-Aubin, J. (1995). Memory for Related and Unrelated Words: Further Evidence on the Influence of Semantic Factors in Immediate Serial Recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 48(2), 384-404. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1080/14640749508401396
Poirier, M., & Saint-Aubin, J. (1996). Immediate serial recall, word frequency, item identity and item position. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 50(4), 408-412. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1037/1196-1961.50.4.408
Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience, 139(1), 23-38. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005
R Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from http:// www.r-project.org/
Roodenrys, S., Hulme, C., Alban, J., Ellis, A. W., & Brown, G. D. A. (1994). Effects of word frequency and age of acquisition on short-term memory span. Memory & Cognition, 22(6), 695-701. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.3758/BF03209254
Roodenrys, S., Hulme, C., Lethbridge, A., Hinton, M., & Nimmo, L. M. (2002). Word-frequency and phonological-neighborhood effects on verbal short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(6), 1019-1034. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393. 28.6.1019
Rouder, J. N., & Morey, R. D. (2012). Default Bayes Factors for Model Selection in Regression. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 47(6), 877-903. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2012.734737
Saint-Aubin, J., & Poirier, M. (1999). Semantic Similarity and Immediate Serial Recall: Is There a Detrimental Effect on Order Information? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 52(2), 367-394. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1080/713755814
Saito, S. (2001). The phonological loop and memory for rhythms: An individual differences approach. Memory, 9(4-6), 313-322. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1080/09658210143000164
Salamé, P., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Effects of background music on phonological short-term memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 41(1), 107-122. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1080/14640748908402355
Schendel, Z. A., & Palmer, C. (2007). Suppression effects on musical and verbal memory. Memory & Cognition, 35(4), 640-650. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/ BF03193302
Schulze, K., Dowling, W. J., & Tillmann, B. (2012). Working Memory for Tonal and Atonal Sequences during a Forward and a Backward Recognition Task. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29(3), 255-267. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1525/ mp.2012.29.3.255
Siedenburg, K., Mativetsky, S., & McAdams, S. (2016). Auditory and verbal memory in North Indian tabla drumming. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 26(4), 327-336. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/ pmu0000163
Siedenburg, K., & McAdams, S. (2017). The role of long-term familiarity and attentional maintenance in short-term memory for timbre. Memory, 25(4), 550-564. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/0965 8211.2016.1197945
Thorn, A. S. C., Gathercole, S. E., & Frankish, C. R. (2005). Redintegration and the benefits of long-term knowledge in verbal short-term memory: An evaluation of Schweickert's (1993) multinomial processing tree model. Cognitive Psychology, 50(2), 133-158. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.07.001
Tillmann, B., Lévêque, Y., Fornoni, L., Albouy, P., & Caclin, A. (2016). Impaired short-term memory for pitch in congenital amusia. Brain Research, 1640, 251-263. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1016/j.brainres.2015.10.035
Tubach, J. L., & Boë, L. J. (1990). Un corpus de transcription phonétique. Moulineaux, France: Telecom.
Williamson, V. J., Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (2010). Musicians' and nonmusicians' short-term memory for verbal and musical sequences: Comparing phonological similarity and pitch proximity. Memory & Cognition, 38(2), 163-175. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.2.163