Attout, L., Fias, W., Salmon, E., & Majerus, S. (2014). Common neural substrates for ordinal representation in short-term memory, numerical and alphabetical cognition. PLoS ONE, 9, e92049. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092049
Berger, J. O., & Wolpert, R. L. (1988). The likelihood principle (2nd ed.). Hayward, CA: Institute of Mathematical Statistics.
Botvinick, M., & Watanabe, T. (2007). From numerosity to ordinal rank: A gain-field model of serial order representation in cortical working memory. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 8636–8642. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2110-07.2007
Brown, G. D., Neath, I., & Chater, N. (2007). A temporal ratio model of memory. Psychological Review, 114, 539–576. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.3.539
Brown, G. D. A., Preece, T., & Hulme, C. (2000). Oscillator-based memory for serial order. Psychological Review, 107, 127–181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.127
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106, 551–581. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.551
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (2006). A revised model of short-term memory and long-term learning of verbal sequences. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 627–652. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.08.005
Chechlacz, M., Rotshtein, P., & Humphreys, G. W. (2014). Neuronal substrates of Corsi block span: Lesion symptom mapping analyses in relation to attentional competition and spatial bias. Neuropsychologia, 64, 240–251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.038
Chochon, F., Cohen, L., van de Moortele, P. F., & Dehaene, S. (1999).
Differential contributions of the left and right inferior parietal lobules to number processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 617–630. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892999563689
Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2011). Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34, 569–599. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731
Cumming, N., Page, M., & Norris, D. (2003). Testing a positional model of the Hebb effect. Memory, 11, 43–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/741938175
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371
Dienes, Z. (2011). Bayesian versus orthodox statistics: Which side are you on? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 274–290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691611406920
Dienes, Z. (2014). Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Frontiers in Psychology: Quantitative Psychology and Measurement, 5, 781. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00781
Dienes, Z. (2016). How Bayes factors change scientific practice. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 72, 78–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2015.10.003
Farrell, S., Hurlstone, M. J., & Lewandowsky, S. (2013). Sequential dependencies in recall of sequences: Filling in the blanks. Memory & Cognition, 41, 938–952. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0310-0
Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., & Orban, G. A. (2003).
Parietal representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 47–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892903321107819
Ginsburg, V., van Dijck, J. P., Previtali, P., Fias, W., & Gevers, W. (2014). The impact of verbal working memory on number-space associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 976–986. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036378
Guida, A., Leroux, A., Lavielle-Guida, M., & Noël, Y. (2016). A SPoARC in the dark: Spatialization in verbal immediate memory. Cognitive Science, 40, 2108–2121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12316
Guida, A., Megreya, A. M., Lavielle-Guida, M., Noël, Y., Mathy, F., van Dijck, J. P., & Abrahamse, E. (2018). Spatialization in working memory is related to literacy and reading direction: Culture “literarily” directs our thoughts. Cognition, 175, 96–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.013
Hartley, T., Hurlstone, M. J., & Hitch, G. J. (2016). Effects of rhythm on memory for spoken sequences: A model and tests of its stimulus-driven mechanism. Cognitive Psychology, 87, 135–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.05.001
Hebb, D. O. (1959). Intelligence, brain function and the theory of mind.
Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 82, 260–275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/82.2.260
Hebb, D. O. (1961). Distinctive features of learning in the higher animal.
In J. E. Delafresnaye (Ed.), Brain mechanisms and learning (pp. 37–46).
New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Henson, R. N. A. (1998). Short-term memory for serial order: The start-end model. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 73–137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0685
Henson, R., Hartley, T., Burgess, N., Hitch, G., & Flude, B. (2003). Selective interference with verbal short-term memory for serial order information: A new paradigm and tests of a timing-signal hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 1307–1334. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02724980244000747
Henson, R. N. A., Norris, D., Page, M. P. A., & Baddeley, A. (1996). Unchained memory: Error patterns rule out chaining models of immediate serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 80–115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713755612
Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 435–448. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1684
Hurlstone, M. J., Hitch, G. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2014). Memory for serial order across domains: An overview of the literature and directions for future research. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 339–373. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034221
Jeffreys, H. (1961). The theory of probability. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Kahana, M. J., Mollison, M. V., & Addis, K. M. (2010). Positional cues in serial learning: The spin-list technique. Memory & Cognition, 38, 92–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.1.92
Lashley, K. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior (pp. 112–136). New York, NY: Wiley.
Lee, M. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2013). Bayesian cognitive modeling: A practical course. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139087759
Lewandowsky, S., & Farrell, S. (2008). Phonological similarity in serial recall: Constraints on theories of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 429–448. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.01.005
Majerus, S. (2019). Verbal working memory and the phonological buffer: The question of serial order. Cortex, 112, 122–133.
Majerus, S., & Attout, L. (2018). Working memory for serial order and numerical cognition: What kind of association? In A. Henik & W. Fias (Eds.), Heterogeneity of function in numerical cognition (pp. 409–403). London, UK: Elsevier. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-811529-9.00019-4
Majerus, S., D’Argembeau, A., Martinez Perez, T., Belayachi, S., Van der Linden, M., Collette, F., … Maquet, P. (2010). The commonality of neural networks for verbal and visual short-term memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2570–2593. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21378
Majerus, S., Martinez Perez, T., & Oberauer, K. (2012). Two distinct origins of long-term learning effects in verbal short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 38–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.07.006
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Van der Linden, M., Albouy, G., Salmon, E., Sterpenich, V., … Maquet, P. (2006). The left intraparietal sulcus and verbal short-term memory: Focus of attention or serial order? NeuroImage, 32, 880–891. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.048
Majerus, S., Van der Linden, M., Mulder, L., Meulemans, T., & Peters, F. (2004). Verbal short-term memory reflects the sublexical organization of the phonological language network: Evidence from an incidental phonotactic learning paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 297–306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.05.002
Marshuetz, C., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., & Noll, D. C. (2006). Working memory for order and the parietal cortex: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroscience, 139, 311–316. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.071
Marshuetz, C., Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., DeGutis, J., & Chenevert, T. L. (2000). Order information in working memory: FMRI evidence for parietal and prefrontal mechanisms. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 130–144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/08989290051137459
Nakayama, M., & Saito, S. (2017). Position-element frequency learning is dissociable from Hebb repetition learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 94, 235–253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.11.007
Oberauer, K., & Lin, H.-Y. (2017). An interference model of visual working memory. Psychological Review, 124, 21–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/rev0000044
Osth, A. F., & Dennis, S. (2015). The fill-in effect in serial recall can be obscured by omission errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 1447–1455. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000113
Page, M. P., & Norris, D. (2009). A model linking immediate serial recall, the Hebb repetition effect and the learning of phonological word forms. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences, 364, 3737–3753. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0173
Pattamadilok, C., Lafontaine, H., Morais, J., & Kolinsky, R. (2010).
Auditory word serial recall benefits from orthographic dissimilarity. Language and Speech, 53, 321–341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0023830910371450
Pinel, P., Dehaene, S., Rivière, D., & LeBihan, D. (2001). Modulation of parietal activation by semantic distance in a number comparison task. NeuroImage, 14, 1013–1026. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0913
Rao, S. M., Mayer, A. R., & Harrington, D. L. (2001). The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 317–323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/85191
Schönbrodt, F. D. (2016). BFDA: Bayes factor design analysis package for R. Retrieved from https://github.com/nicebread/BFDA
Schönbrodt, F. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2018). Bayes factor design analysis: Planning for compelling evidence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 128–142. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1230-y
van Dijck, J. P., Abrahamse, E. L., Majerus, S., & Fias, W. (2013). Spatial attention interacts with serial-order retrieval from verbal working memory. Psychological Science, 24, 1854–1859. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797613479610
van Dijck, J.-P., & Fias, W. (2011). A working memory account for spatial-numerical associations. Cognition, 119, 114–119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.013
Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2007). A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 779–804. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03194105
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Lee, M. D., Lodewyckx, T., & Iverson, G. J. (2008).
Bayesian versus frequentist inference. In H. Hoijtink, I. Klugkist, & P. A. Boelen (Eds.), Bayesian evaluation of informative hypotheses (pp. 181–207). New York, NY: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-09612-4_9
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Love, J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., … Morey, R. D. (2018). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 58–76.
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Verhagen, J., Ly, A., Bakker, M., Lee, M. D., Matzke, D., … Morey, R. D. (2015). A power fallacy. Behavior Research Methods, 47, 913–917. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0517-4
Zhang, Q., Li, S., Wang, X., & Che, X. (2016). The effects of direction similarity in visual working memory: Behavioural and event-related potential studies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 69, 1812–1830. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1100206