Social learning; response facilitation; Behavioural contagion; Model-based biases; Material culture; Token exchange
Abstract :
[en] Animals use social information, available from conspecifics, to learn and express novel and adaptive behaviours. Amongst social learning mechanisms, response facilitation occurs when observing a demonstrator performing a behaviour temporarily increases the probability that the observer will perform the same behaviour shortly after. We studied “robbing and bartering” (RB), two behaviours routinely displayed by free-ranging long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) at Uluwatu Temple, Bali, Indonesia. When robbing, a monkey steals an inedible object from a visitor and may use this object as a token by exchanging it for food with the temple staff (bartering). We tested whether the expression of RB-related behaviours could be explained by response facilitation and was influenced by model-based biases (i.e. dominance rank, age, experience and success of the demonstrator). We compared video-recorded focal samples of 44 witness individuals (WF) immediately after they observed an RB-related event performed by group members, and matched-control focal samples (MCF) of the same focal subjects, located at similar distance from former demonstrators (N = 43 subjects), but in the absence of any RB-related demonstrations. We found that the synchronized expression of robbing and bartering could be explained by response facilitation. Both behaviours occurred significantly more often during WF than during MCF. Following a contagion-like effect, the rate of robbing behaviour displayed by the witness increased with the cumulative rate of robbing behaviour performed by demonstrators, but this effect was not found for the bartering behaviour. The expression of RB was not influenced by model-based biases. Our results support the cultural nature of the RB practice in the Uluwatu macaques.
Disciplines :
Zoology
Author, co-author :
Brotcorne, Fany ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Biologie, Ecologie et Evolution > Biologie du comportement - Ethologie et psychologie animale
Holzner, Anna; Universiti Sains Malaysia > School of Biological Sciences
Jorge‑Sales, Lucía; Miku Conservación AC México > Primate Conservation and Sustainable Development
Gunst, Noëlle; University of Lethbridge, Canada > Department of Psychology
Hambuckers, Alain ; Université de Liège - ULiège > Département de Biologie, Ecologie et Evolution > Biologie du comportement - Ethologie et psychologie animale
Wandia, Nengah; Udayana University > Faculty of Veterinary Medicine
Leca, Jean-Baptiste; University of Lethbridge > Department of Psychology
Language :
English
Title :
Social influence on the expression of robbing and bartering behaviours in Balinese long‑tailed macaques
Addessi E, Crescimbene L, Visalberghi E (2007) Do capuchin monkeys use tokens as symbols? Proc R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci 274:2579–2585. 10.1098/rspb.2007.0726
Agresti A (2007) Introduction to categorical data analysis. Wiley, New York
Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227–267
Amici F, Aureli F, Call J (2014) Response facilitation in the four great apes: Is there a role for empathy? Primates 55:113–118. 10.1007/s10329-013-0375-1
Barrett L, Henzi P (2005) The social nature of primate cognition. Proc Biol Sci 272:1865–1875. 10.1098/rspb.2005.3200
Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67:1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Bevacqua S, Cerasti E, Falcone R, Cervelloni M, Brunamonti E, Ferraina S, Genovesio A (2013) Macaque monkeys can learn token values from human models through vicarious reward. PLoS ONE 8:e59961. 10.1371/journal.pone.0059961
Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Stevens MHH, White J-SS (2009) Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol Evol 24:127–135. 10.1016/j.tree.2008.10.008
Bono AEJ, Whiten A, van Schaik C, Krützen M, Eichenberger F, Schnider A, van de Waal E (2018) Payoff- and sex-biased social learning interact in a wild primate population. Curr Biol 28:2800–2805.e2804. 10.1016/j.cub.2018.06.015
Botting J, Whiten A, Grampp M, van de Waal E (2018) Field experiments with wild primates reveal no consistent dominance-based bias in social learning. Anim Behav 136:1–12. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.11.025
Brosnan SF, de Waal FBM (2004a) A concept of value during experimental exchange in brown capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Folia Primatol 75:317–330. 10.1159/000080209
Brosnan SF, de Waal FBM (2004b) Socially learned preferences for differentially rewarded tokens in the brown capuchin monkey (Cebus apella). J Comp Psychol 118:133–139. 10.1037/0735-7036.118.2.133
Brotcorne F (2014) Behavioral ecology of commensal long-tailed macaque (Macaca fascicularis) populations in Bali, Indonesia: impact of anthropic factors. PhD thesis, University of Liège
Brotcorne F, Fuentes A, Wandia IN, Beudels-Jamar R, Huynen M-C (2015) Changes in activity patterns and intergroup relationships after a significant mortality event in commensal long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) in Bali, Indonesia. Int J Primatol 36:548–566. 10.1007/s10764-015-9841-5
Brotcorne F, Giraud G, Gunst N, Fuentes A, Wandia IN, Beudels-Jamar RC, Poncin P, Huynen MC, Leca J-B (2017) Intergroup variation in robbing and bartering by long-tailed macaques at Uluwatu Temple (Bali, Indonesia). Primates 58:505–516. 10.1007/s10329-017-0611-1
Brown GR, Almond REA, Bergen YV (2004) Begging, stealing, and offering: food transfer in nonhuman primates. In: Slater PJB, Rosenblatt JS, Roper TJ, Snowdon CT, Brockmann HJ, Naguib M (eds) Advances in the study of behavior, vol 34. Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego, pp 265–295
Byrne RW (2002) Imitation of novel complex actions: what does the evidence from animals mean? Adv Stud Behav 31:77–105. 10.1016/S0065-3454(02)80006-7
Byrne RW, Russon AE (1998) Learning by imitation: a hierarchical approach. Behav Brain Sci 21:667–721
Chauhan A, Pirta RS (2010) Agonistic interactions between humans and two species of monkeys (Rhesus monkey Macaca mulatta and Hanuman langur Semnopithecus entellus) in Shimla, Himachal Pradesh. J Psychol 1:9–14. 10.1080/09764224.2010.11885439
Coussi-Korbel S, Fragaszy DM (1995) On the relation between social dynamics and social learning. Anim Behav 50:1441–1453. 10.1016/0003-3472(95)80001-8
de Waal FBM, Yoshihara D (1983) Reconciliation and redirected affection in rhesus monkeys. Behaviour 85:224–241. 10.1163/156853983X00237
Delignette-Muller ML, Dutang C (2015) fitdistrplus: an R package for fitting distributions. J Stat Softw 64:1–34. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v064.i04
Duboscq J, Romano V, MacIntosh A, Sueur C (2016) Social information transmission in animals: lessons from studies of diffusion. Front Psychol 7:1147. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01147
Duranton C, Gaunet F (2016) Behavioural synchronization from an ethological perspective: overview of its adaptive value. Adapt Behav 24:181–191. 10.1177/1059712316644966
Ferrari SL, Cribari-Neto F (2004) Beta regression for modelling rates and proportions. J Appl Stat 31:799–815. 10.1080/0266476042000214501
Ferrari PF, Maiolini C, Addessi E, Fogassi L, Visalberghi E (2005) The observation and hearing of eating actions activates motor programs related to eating in macaque monkeys. Behav Brain Res 161:95–101. 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.01.009
Fooden J (1995) Systematic review of southeast Asian longtail macaques, Macaca fascicularis, (Raffles, [1821]). Fieldiana Zool 81:1–206. 10.5962/bhl.title.3456
Forstmeier W, Scheielzeth H (2011) Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: overestimated effect sizes and the winner's curse. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 65:47–55. 10.1007/s00265-010-1038-5
Fox GA (1993) Failure-time analysis: emergence, flowering, survivorship, and other waiting times. In: Scheiner SM, Gurevitch J (eds) Design and analysis of ecological experiments. Chapman & Hall, New York, pp 253–289
Fragaszy DM, Perry S (2003) The biology of traditions: models and evidence. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Fragaszy D, Visalberghi E (2004) Socially biased learning in monkeys. Learn Behav 32:24–35
Fragaszy DM, Eshchar Y, Visalberghi E, Resende B, Laity K, Izar P (2017) Synchronized practice helps bearded capuchin monkeys learn to extend attention while learning a tradition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 114:7798–7805. 10.1073/pnas.1621071114
Fuentes A, Southern M, Suaryana KG (2005) Monkey forests and human landscapes: is extensive sympatry sustainable for Homo sapiens and Macaca fascicularis on Bali? In: Patterson JD, Wallis J (eds) Commensalism and conflict: the human-primate interface American Society of Primatologists, vol 4. Norman, Oklahoma, pp 168–195
Fuentes A, Rompis ALT, Arta Putra IGA, Watiniasih NL, Suatha IK, Soma IG, Wandia IN, Harya Putra I, Stephenson R, Selamet W (2011) Macaque behavior at the human-monkey interface: the activity and demography of semi-free ranging Macaca fascicularis at Padangtegal, Bali, Indonesia. In: Gumert MD, Fuentes A, Jones-Engel L (eds) Monkeys on the edge. Ecology and management of long-tailed macaques and their interface with humans. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 159–179
Galef JBG (1995) Why behaviour patterns that animals learn socially are locally adaptive. Anim Behav 49:1325–1334. 10.1006/anbe.1995.0164
Galef BG (2013) Imitation and local enhancement: detrimental effects of consensus definitions on analyses of social learning in animals. Behav Processes 100:123–130. 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.07.026
Gray B (2015) cmprsk: subdistribution analysis of competing risks. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=cmprsk
Heyes CM (1994) Social learning in animals: categories and mechanisms. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 69:207–231. 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1994.tb01506.x
Hopper LM, Schapiro SJ, Lambeth SP, Brosnan SF (2011) Chimpanzees' socially maintained food preferences indicate both conservatism and conformity. Anim Behav 81:1195–1202. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.03.002
Hopper LM, Kurtycz LM, Ross SR, Bonnie KE (2015) Captive chimpanzee foraging in a social setting: a test of problem solving, flexibility, and spatial discounting. PeerJ 3:e833. 10.7717/peerj.833
Hoppitt W, Laland KN (2008) Social processes influencing learning in animals: a review of the evidence. Adv Study Behav 38:105–165. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(08)00003-X
Hoppitt W, Blackburn L, Laland KN (2007) Response facilitation in the domestic fowl. Anim Behav 73:229–238. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.05.013
Horner V, Proctor D, Bonnie KE, Whiten A, de Waal FB (2010) Prestige affects cultural learning in chimpanzees. PLoS ONE 5:e10625. 10.1371/journal.pone.0010625
Jahn-Eimermacher A, Lasarzik I, Raber J (2011) Statistical analysis of latency outcomes in behavioral experiments. Behav Brain Res 221:271–275. 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.007
Kaburu S, Marty P, Beisner B, Balasubramaniam K, Bliss-Moreau E, Kaur K, Mohan L, McCowan B (2019) Rates of human-macaque interactions affect grooming behavior among urban-dwelling rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Am J Phys Anthropol 168:92–103. 10.1002/ajpa.23722
Kendal R, Hopper LM, Whiten A, Brosnan SF, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ, Hoppitt W (2015) Chimpanzees copy dominant and knowledgeable individuals: implications for cultural diversity. Evol Hum Behav 36:65–72. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.09.002
Kopp S, Graeser O (2006) Imitation learning and response facilitation in embodied agents. In: Gratch J, Young M, Aylett R, Ballin D, Olivier P (eds) Intelligent virtual agents, Berlin. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, pp 28–41
Leca J-B, Gunst N, Huffman MA (2008) Food provisioning and stone handling tradition in Japanese macaques: a comparative study of ten troops. Am J Primatol 70:803–813. 10.1002/ajp.20551
Leca J-B, Gunst N, Huffman MA (2010) Indirect social influence in the maintenance of the stone-handling tradition in Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata. Anim Behav 79:117–126. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.035
Mangalam M, Singh M (2013) Flexibility in food extraction techniques in urban free-ranging bonnet macaques Macaca radiata. PLoS ONE 8:e85497. 10.1371/journal.pone.0085497
Martin P, Bateson P (1993) Measuring behaviour. An Introductory guide, 2 edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
McKinney T (2015) A classification system for describing anthropogenic influence on nonhuman primate populations. Am J Primatol 77:715–726. 10.1002/ajp.22395
McLennan MR, Spagnoletti N, Hockings KJ (2017) The implications of primate behavioral flexibility for sustainable human–primate coexistence in anthropogenic habitats. Int J Primatol 38:105–121. 10.1007/s10764-017-9962-0
Meunier H, Petit O, Deneubourg J-L (2008) Social facilitation of fur rubbing behavior in white-faced capuchins. Am J Primatol 70:161–168. 10.1002/ajp.20468
Nakayama K (2004) Observing conspecifics scratching induces a contagion of scratching in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). J Comp Psychol 118:20–24. 10.1037/0735-7036.118.1.20
Nuechterlein GL, Storer RW (1982) The pair-formation displays of the western grebe. Condor 84:351–369. 10.2307/1367437
Palagi E, Norscia I, Demuru E (2014) Yawn contagion in humans and bonobos: emotional affinity matters more than species. PeerJ 2:e519. 10.7717/peerj.519
R Core Team (2013) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna
Radhakrishna S, Huffman MA, Sinha A (2013) The macaque connection. Cooperation and conflict between humans and macaques. Springer, New York
Sinha A (2005) Not in their genes: phenotypic flexibility, behavioural traditions and cultural evolution in wild bonnet macaques. J Biosci (Bangalore) 30:51–64
Tolman CW (1964) Social facilitation of feeding behaviour in the domestic chick. Anim Behav 12:245–251. 10.1016/0003-3472(64)90008-9
Tomasello M (1998) Emulation learning and cultural learning. Behav Brain Sci 21:703–704. 10.1017/S0140525X98441748
van de Waal E, Renevey N, Favre CM, Bshary R (2010) Selective attention to philopatric models causes directed social learning in wild vervet monkeys. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 277:2105–2111. 10.1098/rspb.2009.2260
Videan EN, Fritz J, Schwandt M, Howell S (2005) Neighbor effect: evidence of affiliative and agonistic social contagion in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am J Primatol 66:131–144. 10.1002/ajp.20133
Visalberghi E, Addessi E (2000) Seeing group members eating a familiar food enhances the acceptance of novel foods in capuchin monkeys. Anim Behav 60:69–76. 10.1006/anbe.2000.1425
Visalberghi E, Addessi E (2001) Acceptance of novel foods in capuchin monkeys: do specific social facilitation and visual stimulus enhancement play a role? Anim Behav 62:567–576. 10.1006/anbe.2001.1787
Whiten A (1998) Imitation of the sequential structure of actions by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 112:270–281. 10.1037/0735-7036.112.3.270
Whiten A (2000) Primate culture and social learning. Cogn Sci 24:477–508. 10.1207/s15516709cog2403_6
Wrangham RW, Koops K, Machanda ZP, Worthington S, Bernard AB, Brazeau NF, Donovan R, Rosen J, Wilke C, Otali E, Muller MN (2016) Distribution of a chimpanzee social custom is explained by matrilineal relationship rather than conformity. Curr Biol 26:3033–3037. 10.1016/j.cub.2016.09.005
Zentall TR (1996) An analysis of imitative learning in animals. In: Heyes CM, Galef BG (eds) Social learning in animals. The roots of culture. Academic Press, New York, pp 221–241
Zupan M, Rehn T, de Oliveira D, Malovrh Š, Keeling L (2019) Individual play patterns stimulated by a familiar object are group-driven. Sci Rep 9:6092. 10.1038/s41598-019-42382-9