2018 - Puttaert et al - 2018 - Subjectively-defined optimal-non-optimal time of day modulates controlled but not automatic retrieval processes in verbal memory.pdf
Adam, S., Van der Linden, M., Collette, F., Lemauvais, L., & Salmon, E. (2005). Further exploration of controlled and automatic memory processes in early Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 19, 420–427. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.420
Åkerstedt, T., & Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. International Journal of Neuroscience, 52, 29–37. https://doi.org/10.3109/00207459008994241
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck depression inventory-II(pp. 1–82). San Antonio: TX Psychol.
Bodenhausen, G. V. (1990). Stereotypes as judgmental heuristics: Evidence of circadian variations in discrimination. Psychological Science, 1, 319–322. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00226.x
Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., & Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research, 28, 193–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4
Correa, Á., Molina, E., & Sanabria, D. (2014). Effects of chronotype and time of day on the vigilance decrement during simulated driving. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 67, 113–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2014.02.020
Delpouve, J., Schmitz, R., & Peigneux, P. (2014). Implicit learning is better at subjectively defined non-optimal time of day. Cortex, 58, 18–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.05.006
Deltour, J. J.; Echelle de vocabulaire de Mill Hill de JC Raven. (1993). Adaptation française et normes européennes du Mill Hill et du Standard Progressive Matrices de Raven (PM38). Braine-le-Château, Belgium: L'Application des Techniques Modernes.
Destrebecqz, A., Peigneux, P., Laureys, S., Degueldre, C., Del Fiore, G., Aerts, J., … Maquet, P. (2005). The neural correlates of implicit and explicit sequence learning: Interacting networks revealed by the process dissociation procedure. Learning & Memory, 12, 480–490. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.95605
Dienes, Z. (2011). Bayesian versus orthodox statistics: Which side are you on? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 274–290. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611406920
Dijk, D.-J., & von Schantz, M. (2005). Timing and consolidation of human sleep, wakefulness, and performance by a symphony of oscillators. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 20, 279–290. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730405278292
Dinges, D. F., & Powell, J. W. (1985). Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 17, 652–655. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200977
Edwards, B., Waterhouse, J., & Reilly, T. (2007). The effects of circadian rhythmicity and time-awake on a simple motor task. Chronobiology International, 24, 1109–1124. https://doi.org/10.1080/07420520701795316
Ellis, B. W., Johns, M. W., Lancaster, R., Raptopoulos, P., Angelopoulos, N., & Priest, R. G. (1981). The St. Mary's Hospital sleep questionnaire: A study of reliability. Sleep, 9, 3–97.
Espinosa-Garcia, M., Vaquero, J. M., Milliken, B., & Tudela, P. (2017). Recollection and familiarity for words and faces: A study comparing Remember-Know judgments and the Process Dissociation Procedure. Memory, 25, 19–34. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2015.1120310
Gardiner, J. M., Ramponi, C., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (1998). Experiences of remembering, knowing and guessing. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1997.0321
Graw, P., Krauchi, K., Knoblauch, V., Wirz-Justice, A., & Cajochen, C. (2004). Circadian and wake-dependent modulation of fastest and slowest reaction times during the psychomotor vigilance task. Physiology & Behavior, 80, 695–701. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2003.12.004
Hasher, L., Chung, C., May, C. P., & Foong, N. (2002). Age, time of testing, and proactive interference. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 200–207. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0087397
Hasher, L., Goldstein, D., & May, C. P. (2005). It's about time: Circadian rhythms, memory, and aging. In C. Izawa & N. Ohta (Eds.), Human learning and memory: Advances in theory and application: The 4th Tsukuba International Conference on Memory (pp. 199–217). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Horne, J. A., & Östberg, O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology, 2, 97–110.
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90025-F
Kantermann, T., & Eastman, C. I. (2018). Circadian phase, circadian period and chronotype are reproducible over months. Chronobiology International, 35, 280–288. https://doi.org/10.1080/07420528.2017.1400979
Kantermann, T., Sung, H., & Burgess, H. J. (2015). Comparing the morningness-eveningness questionnaire and Munich chronotype questionnaire to the dim light melatonin onset. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 30, 449–453. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730415597520
Knight, R. G. (1998). Controlled and automatic memory process in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex, 34, 427–435. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70765-2
Lotze, M., Wittmann, M., von Steinbüchel, N., Pöppel, E., & Roenneberg, T. (1999). Daily rhythm of temporal resolution in the auditory system. Cortex, 35, 89–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70787-1
May, C. P., & Hasher, L. (1998). Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 363–379.
May, C. P., & Hasher, L. (2017). Synchrony affects performance for older but not younger neutral-type adults. Timing & Time Perception, 5, 129–148. https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-00002087
May, C. P., Hasher, L., & Foong, N. (2005). Implicit memory, age, and time of day: Paradoxical priming effects. Psychological Science, 16, 96–100. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00788.x
May, C. P., Hasher, L., & Stoltzfus, E. R. (1993). Optimal time of day and the magnitude of age differences in memory. Psychological Science, 4, 326–330. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00573.x
Natale, V., Alzani, A., & Cicogna, P. C. (2003). Cognitive efficiency and circadian typologies: A diurnal study. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1089–1105. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00320-3
Ramírez, C., Talamantes, J., García, A., Morales, M., Valdez, P., & Menna-Barreto, L. (2006). Circadian rhythms in phonological and visuospatial storage components of working memory. Biological Rhythm Research, 37, 433–441. https://doi.org/10.1080/09291010600870404
Rothen, N., & Meier, B. (2016). Time of day affects implicit memory for unattended stimuli. Consciousness and Cognition, 46, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.09.012
Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., & Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: Circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 755–789. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290701754158
Smith, J. A., & Knight, R. G. (2002). Memory processing in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia, 40, 666–682. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00137-3
Weiskrantz, L., & Warrington, E. K. (1975). The problem of the amnesic syndrome in man and animal. The Hippocampus, 2, 283–291.
West, R., Murphy, K. J., Armilio, M. L., Craik, F. I. M., & Stuss, D. T. (2002). Effects of time of day on age differences in working memory. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57, 3–10. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/57.1.P3
Yang, L., Hasher, L., & Wilson, D. E. (2007). Synchrony effects in automatic and controlled retrieval. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 51–56. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194027
Yonelinas, A. P., & Jacoby, L. L. (2012). The process-dissociation approach two decades later: Convergence, boundary conditions, and new directions. Memory & Cognition, 40, 663–680. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0205-5
Yoon, C., May, C. P., & Hasher, L. (1999). Aging, circadian arousal patterns and cognition. In N. Schwarz, D. Park, B. Knauper & S. Sudman (Eds.), Aging, cognition and self reports (pp. 117–143). Washington, DC: Psychological Press.