Beery, K. E., & Beery, N. A. (2004). Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration: Administration, scoring, and teaching manual (5th ed). Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson Beery.
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z., & St George, M. (2000). The neurocognitive profile of Williams Syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 7-29.
Bellugi, U., Sabo, H., & Vaid, J. (1988). Spatial deficits in children with Williams Syndrome. In Stiles-Davis J., Kritchevsky M., & Bellugi U. (Eds.), Spatial cognition: Brain bases and development, 298, 273). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Bellugi, U., Wang, P. P., & Jernigan, T. L. (1994). Williams syndrome: An unusual neuropsychological profile. In Broman S. H., & Grafman J. (Eds.), Atypical cognitive deficits in developmental disorders: Implications for brain function, 56, 23). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Bernardino, I., Mouga, S., Castelo-Branco, M., & van Asselen, M. (2013). Egocentric and allocentric spatial representations in Williams Syndrome. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 54-62. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617712000963.
Bertrand, J., Mervis, C. B., & Isenberg, J. D. (1997). Drawing by children with Williams syndrome: A developmental perspective. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 41-67. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649709540667.
Bihrle, A. M., Bellugi, U., Delis, D., & Marks, S. (1989). Seeing either the forest or the trees: Dissociation in visuospatial processing. Brain and Cognition, 11, 37-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(89)90003-1.
Broadbent, H., Farran, E. K., & Tolmie, A. (2014). Egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies in Williams syndrome and typical development. Developmental Science, 17, 920-934. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12176.
Broadbent, H., Farran, E. K., & Tolmie, A. (2015). Sequential egocentric navigation and reliance on landmarks in Williams syndrome and typical development. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 216. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00216.
Bullens, J., Igli, K., Berthoz, A., Postma, A., & Rondi-Reig, L. (2010). Developmental time course of the acquisition of sequential egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 337-350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.05.010.
Carlson, L. A., West, R., Taylor, H. A., & Herndon, R. W. (2002). Neural correlates of spatial term use. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1391-1408. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.28.6.1391.
Carlson-Radvansky, L. A., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Inhibition accompanies reference-frame selection. Psychological Science, 9, 386-391.
Coie, J. D., Costanzo, P. R., & Farnill, D. (1973). Specific transitions in the development of spatial perspective-taking ability. Developmental Psychology, 9(2), 167-177. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035062.
Dykens, E. M., Rosner, B. A., & Ly, T. M. (2001). Drawings by individuals with Williams syndrome: Are people different from shapes? American Journal of Mental Retardation: AJMR, 106, 94-107. https://doi.org/10.1352/0895-8017(2001)1060094:DBIWWS2.0.CO;2.
Farran, E. K., Blades, M., Boucher, J., & Tranter, L. J. (2010). How do individuals with Williams syndrome learn a route in a real-world environment? Developmental Science, 13, 454-468. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00894.x.
Farran, E. K., & Jarrold, C. (2003). Visuospatial cognition in Williams syndrome: Reviewing and accounting for the strengths and weaknesses in performance. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 173-200. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326942DN231&2-8.
Farran, E. K., & Jarrold, C. (2005). Evidence for unusual spatial location coding in Williams syndrome: An explanation for the local bias in visuo-spatial construction tasks? Brain and Cognition, 59, 159-172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2005.05.011.
Farran, E. K., Jarrold, C., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Block design performance in the Williams syndrome phenotype: A problem with mental imagery? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42, 719-728.
Fisher, M. H., Lense, M. D., & Dykens, E. M. (2016). Longitudinal trajectories of intellectual and adaptive functioning in adolescents and adults with Williams syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research: JIDR, 60, 920-932. https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12303.
Galati, G., Lobel, E., Vallar, G., Berthoz, A., Pizzamiglio, L., & Le Bihan, D. (2000). The neural basis of egocentric and allocentric coding of space in humans: A functional magnetic resonance study. Experimental Brain Research, 133, 156-164.
Hayward, W. G., & Tarr, M. J. (1995). Spatial language and spatial representation. Cognition, 55, 39-84.
Heiz, J., & Barisnikov, K. (2015). How flexible is the visuospatial reference system in children aged 4 to 12? Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology, 5, 51-62. https://doi.org/10.5539/jedp.v5n2p51.
Heiz, J., Cengic, L., Fernandes, A., & Barisnikov, K. (2015). Body schemas of individuals with Williams syndrome. Genève.
Howard, I. P., & Templeton, W. B. (1966). Human spatial orientation. New York: Wiley.
Howlin, P., Elison, S., Udwin, O., & Stinton, C. (2010). Cognitive, linguistic and adaptive functioning in Williams syndrome: Trajectories from early to middle adulthood. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 23, 322-336. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-3148.2009.00536.x.
Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N., & Sandberg, E. H. (1994). The coding of spatial location in young children. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 115-147. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1994.1014.
Kim, H., & Cameron, C. E. (2016). Implications of visuospatial skills and executive functions for learning mathematics: Evidence from children with autism and Williams Syndrome. AERA Open, 2, 2332858416675124. https://doi.org/10.1177/2332858416675124.
Klatzky, R. L.(1998). Allocentric and egocentric spatial representations: Definitions, distinctions, and interconnections. In Freksa C., Habel C. & Wender K. F. (Eds), Spatial cognition (pp. 1-17). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
Levinson, S. C. (1996). Language and space. Annual Review of Anthropology, 25, 353-382. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.25.1.353.
Lourenco, S. F., & Frick, A. (2013). Remembering where: The origins and early development of spatial memory. In Bauer P. J., & Fivush R. (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on the development of children's memory (pp. 361-393). UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Mandolesi, L., Addona, F., Foti, F., Menghini, D., Petrosini, L., & Vicari, S. (2009). Spatial competences in Williams syndrome: A radial arm maze study. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 27, 205-213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2009.01.004.
Mervis, C. B. (2003). Williams syndrome: 15 years of psychological research. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23 (1-2), 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2003.9651884.
Mervis, C. B. (2009). Language and literacy development of children with Williams Syndrome. Topics in Language Disorders, 29, 149-169. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0b013e3181a72044.
Mervis, C. B., & John, A. E. (2010). Cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with Williams syndrome: Implications for intervention approaches. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C: Seminars in Medical Genetics, 154C, 229-248. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.c.30263.
Mervis, C. B., & Klein-Tasman, B. P. (2000). Williams syndrome: Cognition, personality, and adaptive behavior. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 6, 148-158. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-2779(2000)6:2148::AID-MRDD103.0.CO;2-T.
Nardini, M., Atkinson, J., Braddick, O., & Burgess, N. (2008). Developmental trajectories for spatial frames of reference in Williams syndrome. Developmental Science, 11, 583-595. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00662.x.
Nardini, M., Burgess, N., Breckenridge, K., & Atkinson, J. (2006). Differential developmental trajectories for egocentric, environmental and intrinsic frames of reference in spatial memory. Cognition, 101, 153-172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.09.005.
Newcombe, N., Huttenlocher, J., Drummey, A. B., & Wiley, J. G. (1998). The development of spatial location coding: Place learning and dead reckoning in the second and third years. Cognitive Development, 13, 185-200. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90038-7.
Paterson, S. J., Girelli, L., Butterworth, B., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). Are numerical impairments syndrome specific? Evidence from Williams syndrome and Down's syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 47, 190-204. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01460.x.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1947). La conception de l'espace chez l'enfant. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Raven, J. C., Raven, J., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (2003). Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales (Harcourt Assessment, Vol. Section 1: General Overview). San Antonio, TX.
Rochat, P. (1995). Perceived reachability for self and for others by 3- to 5-year-old children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 317-333. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1995.1014.
Rosner, B. A., Hodapp, R. M., Fidler, D. J., Sagun, J. N., & Dykens, E. M. (2004). Social competence in Persons with Prader-Willi, Williams and Down's Syndromes. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 17, 209-217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-3148.2004.00200.x.
Saj, A., Heiz, J., & Barisnikov, K. (2017). Where is the 'subjective straight ahead' in Williams syndrome? Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 61, 512-518. https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12358.
Semel, E. M., & Rosner, S. R. (2003). Understanding Williams Syndrome: Behavioral patterns and interventions. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Shelton, A. L., & Mcnamara, T. P. (1997). Multiple views of spatial memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 102-106. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210780.
Simon, T. J. (2007). Cognitive characteristics of children with genetic syndromes. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 16, 599-616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2007.03.002.
Taylor, H. A., & Rapp, D. (2004). Where is the donut? Factors influencing spatial reference frame use. Cognitive Processing, 5, 175-188. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-004-0022-2.
Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1996). Perspective in spatial descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 371-391. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0021.
Van Herwegen, J., Farran, E., & Annaz, D. (2011). Item and error analysis on Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices in Williams Syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 93-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.09.005.
Verjat, I. (1994). Confrontation de deux approches de la localisation spatiale. L'année Psychologique, 94, 403-423. https://doi.org/10.3406/psy.1994.28774.
Vicario, C. M., Yates, M. J., & Nicholls, M. E. R. (2013). Shared deficits in space, time, and quantity processing in childhood genetic disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00043.
Volterra, V., Capirci, O., & Cristina, M. (2001). What atypical populations can reveal about language development: The contrast between deafness and Williams syndrome. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 219-239. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960042000067.
Zhou, Y., McBride-Chang, C., & Wong, N. (2014). What is the role of visual skills in learning to read? Frontiers in Psychology, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00776.