American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine. (1995). Recommendations for use of uniform nomenclature pertinent to patients with severe alterations of consciousness. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 76, 205-209.
Boly, M., & Faymonville, M. (2004). Auditory processing in severely brain injured patients: Differences between the minimally conscious state and the persistent vegetative state. Archives of Neurology, 61(2), 233-238.
Boly, M., Faymonville, M. E., Schnakers, C., et al. (2008). Perception of pain in the minimally conscious state with PET activation: An observational study. Lancet Neurology, 7(11), 1013-1020.
Boly, M., & Seth, A. K. (2012). Modes and models in disorders of consciousness science. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 150(2-3), 172-184.
Bruno, M. A., Fernández-Espejo, D., Lehembre, R., et al. (2011). Multimodal neuroimaging in patients with disorders of consciousness showing "functional hemispherectomy" Progress in Brain Research, 193, 323-333.
Bruno, M. A., Majerus, S., Boly, M., et al. (2011). Functional neuroanatomy underlying the clinical subcategorization of minimally conscious state patients. Journal of Neurology, 259(6), 1087-1098.
Bruno, M. A., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Thibaut, A., Moonen, G., & Laureys, S. (2011). From unresponsive wakefulness to minimally conscious PLUS and functional locked-in syndromes: Recent advances in our understanding of disorders of consciousness. Journal of Neurology, 258, 1373-1384.
Cruse, D., Chennu, S., Chatelle, C., et al. (2011). Bedside detection of awareness in the vegetative state: A cohort study. Lancet, 378(9809), 2088-2094.
Damoiseaux, J. S., Rombouts, S., Barkhof, F., et al. (2006). Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(37), 13848-13853.
Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J. P. (2005). Ongoing spontaneous activity controls access to consciousness: A neuronal model for inattentional blindness. PLoS Biology, 3(5), e141.
Di Perri, C., Bastianello, S., Bartsch, A. J., et al. (2013). Limbic hyperconnectivity in the vegetative state. Neurology, 81, 1417-1424.
Di, H., Yu, S. M., Weng, X. C., et al. (2007). Cerebral response to patient's own name in the vegetative and minimally conscious states. Neurology, 68(12), 895-899.
Fernández-Espejo, D., Bekinschtein, T., Monti, M. M., et al. (2011). Diffusion weighted imaging distinguishes the vegetative state from the minimally conscious state. NeuroImage, 54(1), 103-112.
Giacino, J. T., Ashwal, S., Childs, N., et al. (2002). The minimally conscious state: Definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology, 58(3), 349.
Giacino, J., & Kalmar, K. (1997). The vegetative and minimally conscious states: A comparison of clinical features and functional outcome. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 12(4).
Giacino, J. T., Kalmar, K., & Whyte, J. (2004). The JFK coma recovery scale-revised: Measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 85(12), 2020-2029.
Gosseries, O., Schnakers, C., & Ledoux, D. (2011). Automated EEG entropy measurements in coma, vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome and minimally conscious state. Functional Neurology, 26(1), 1-6.
Heine, L., Soddu, A., Gomez, F., et al. (2012). Resting state networks and consciousness: Alterations of multiple resting state network connectivity in physiological, pharmacological and pathological consciousness states. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 295.
Laureys, S., Celesia, G. G., Cohadon, F., et al. (2010). Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: A new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Medicine, 8(1), 68.
Laureys, S., Faymonville, M. E., Degueldre, C., et al. (2000). Auditory processing in the vegetative state. Brain, 123(8), 1589-1601.
Laureys, S., Faymonville, M., & Luxen, A. (2000). Restoration of thalamocortical connectivity after recovery from persistent vegetative state. The Lancet, 355, 1790-1791.
Lechinger, J., Bothe, K., Pichler, G., et al. (2013). CRS-R score in disorders of consciousness is strongly related to spectral EEG at rest. Journal of Neurology, 260, 2348-2356.
Luauté, J., Maucort-Boulch, D., Tell, L., et al. (2010). Long-term outcomes of chronic minimally conscious and vegetative states. Neurology, 75(3), 246-252.
Lull, N., Noé, E., Lull, J. J., et al. (2010). Voxel-based statistical analysis of thalamic glucose metabolism in traumatic brain injury: Relationship with consciousness and cognition. Brain Injury, 24(9), 1098-1107.
Monti, M. M., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Coleman, M. R., et al. (2010). Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. New England Journal of Medicine, 362(7), 579-589.
Naci, L., Cusack, R., Jia, V. Z., et al. (2013). The brain's silent messenger: Using selective attention to decode human thought for brain-based communication. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(22), 9385-9393.
Nakayama, N., Okumura, A., Shinoda, J., et al. (2006). Relationship between regional cerebral metabolism and consciousness disturbance in traumatic diffuse brain injury without large focal lesions: An FDG-PET study with statistical parametric mapping analysis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 77(7), 856-862.
Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Boly, M., et al. (2006). Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science, 313(5792), 1402.
Posner, J. B., Saper, C. B., Schiff, N. D., et al. (2007). Plum and Posner's diagnosis of stupor and coma. (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Rosanova, M., Gosseries, O., Casarotto, S., et al. (2012). Recovery of cortical effective connectivity and recovery of consciousness in vegetative patients. Brain, 135(4), 1308-1320.
Schiff, N. D., Giacino, J. T., Kalmar, K., et al. (2007). Behavioural improvements with thalamic stimulation after severe traumatic brain injury. Nature, 448(7153), 600-603.
Schnakers, C., Perrin, F., Schabus, M., et al. (2008). Voluntary brain processing in disorders of consciousness. Neurology, 71(20), 1614-1620.
Schnakers, C., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Giacino, J. T., et al. (2009). Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: Clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment. Neurology, 9(1), 35.
Seel, R. T., Sherer, M., Whyte, J., et al. (2010). Assessment scales for disorders of consciousness: Evidence-based recommendations for clinical practice and research. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 91(12), 1795-1813.
Smith, S. M., Fox, P. T., Miller, K. L., et al. (2009). Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(31), 13040-13045.
The Multi-Society Task Force of PVS. (1994). Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state. New England Journal of Medicine, 330, 1499-1508.
Thibaut, A., Bruno, M. A., Chatelle, C., et al. (2012). Metabolic activity in external and internal awareness networks in severely brain-damaged patients. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 44(6), 487-497.
Tononi, G. (2008). Consciousness as integrated information: A provisional manifesto. The Biological Bulletin, 215(3), 216-242.
Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Demertzi, A., Schabus, M., et al. (2010). Two distinct neuronal networks mediate the awareness of environment and of self. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(3), 570-578.
Wijdicks, E. F. (2001). The diagnosis of brain death. The New England Journal of Medicine, 344, 1215-1221.
http://www.alis-asso.fr-Association for the Locked-in Syndrome. www.comascience.org-Coma Science Group.