short-term memory; working memory; language; attention; neuroanatomy
Abstract :
[en] Short-term maintenance of verbal information is a core factor of language repetition, especially when reproducing multiple or unfamiliar stimuli. Many models of language processing locate the verbal short-term maintenance function in the left posterior superior temporo-parietal area and its connections with the inferior frontal gyrus. However, research in the field of short-term memory has implicated bilateral fronto-parietal networks, involved in attention and serial order processing, as being critical for the maintenance and reproduction of verbal sequences. We present here an integrative framework aimed at bridging research in the language processing and short-term memory fields. This framework considers verbal short-term maintenance as an emergent function resulting from synchronized and integrated activation in dorsal and ventral language processing networks as well as fronto-parietal attention and serial order processing networks. To-be-maintained item representations are temporarily activated in the dorsal and ventral language processing networks, novel phoneme and word serial order information is proposed to be maintained via a right fronto-parietal serial order processing network, and activation in these different networks is proposed to be coordinated and maintained via a left fronto-parietal attention processing network. This framework provides new perspectives for our understanding of information maintenance at the nonword-, word- and sentence-level as well as of verbal maintenance deficits in case of brain injury.
scite shows how a scientific paper has been cited by providing the context of the citation, a classification describing whether it supports, mentions, or contrasts the cited claim, and a label indicating in which section the citation was made.
Bibliography
Acheson, D. J., and MacDonald, M. C. (2009). Verbal working memory and language production: common approaches to the serial ordering of verbal information. Psychol. Bull. 135, 50-68. doi: 10.1037/a0014411
Arfanakis, K., Haughton, V. M., Carew, J. D., Rogers, B. P., Dempsey, R. J., and Meyerand, M. E. (2002). Diffusion tensor MR imaging in diffuse axonal injury. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 23, 794-802.
Attout, L., Van der Kaa, M. A., George, M., and Majerus, S. (2012). Distinguishing verbal short-term memory and language impairment: the importance of short-term memory for serial order. Aphasiology 26, 355-382. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2011.604303
Awh, E., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Buxton, R. B., Frank, L. R., Love, T., et al. (1999). Rehearsal in spatial working memory: evidence from neuroimaging. Psychol. Sci. 10, 433-437. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00182
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., and Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychol. Rev. 105, 158-173. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.105.1.158
Baddeley, A. D., and Hitch, G. J. (1974). "Working memory, " in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, ed G. H. Bower (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 47-90.
Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G. J., and Allen, R. J. (2009). Working memory and binding in sentence recall. J. Mem. Lang. 61, 438-456. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.05.004
Baddeley, A. D., Lewis, V., and Vallar, G. (1984). Exploring the articulatory loop. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 36, 233-252.
Baddeley, A. D., and Logie, R. H. (1999). "Working memory: the multiple-component model, " in Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control, eds A. Miyake and P. Shah (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 28-61.
Barde, L. H. F., Schwartz, M. F., Chrysikou, E. G., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2010). Reduced short-term memory span in aphasia and susceptibility to interference: ontribution of material-specific maintenance deficits. Neuropsychologia 48, 909-920.
Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., and Camos, V. (2004). Time constraints and resource sharing in adults' working memory spans. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 133, 83-100. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.83
Basso, A., Spinnler, H., Vallar, G., and Zanobio, M. E. (1982). Left hemisphere damage and selective impairment of auditory verbal short-term memory. A case study. Neuropsychologia 20, 263-274. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(82)90101-4
Becker, J. T., MacAndrew, D. K., and Fiez, J. A. (1999). A comment on the functional localization of the phonological storage subsystem of working memory. Brain Cogn. 41, 27-38. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1999.1094
Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., and Conant, L. I. (2009). Where is the semantic system? a critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2767-2796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055
Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Bellgowan, P. S. F., Springer, J. A., Kaufman, J. N., et al. (2000). Human temporal lobe activation by speech and nonspeech sounds. Cereb. Cortex 10, 512-528. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.5.512
Botvinick, M., and Plaut, D. C. (2006). Short-term memory for serial order: a recurrent neural network model. Psychol. Rev. 113, 201-233. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.201
Botvinick, M., and Watanabe, T. (2007). From numerosity to ordinal rank: a gain-field model of serial order representation in cortical working memory. J. Neurosci. 27, 8636-8642. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2110-07.2007
Brahmbhatt, S. B., McAuley, T., and Barch, D. M. (2008). Functional developmental similarities and differences in the neural correlates of verbal and nonverbal working memory tasks. Neuropsychologia 46, 1020-1031. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.11.010
Brener, R. (1940). An experimental investigation of memory span. J. Exp. Psychol. 26, 467-482. doi: 10.1037/h0061096
Bricolo, E., Gianesini, T., Fanini, A., Bundesen, C., and Chelazzi, L. (2002). Serial attention mechanisms in visual search: a direct behavioral demonstration. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 980-993. doi: 10.1162/089892902320474454
Brown, G. D. A., Preece, T., and Hulme, C. (2000). Oscillator-based memory for serial order. Psychol. Rev. 107, 127-181. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.127
Buchsbaum, B. R., and D'Esposito, M. (2008). The search for the phonological store: from loop to convolution. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 762-778. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20501
Buchsbaum, B. R., Baldo, J., Okada, K., Berman, K. F., Dronkers, N. F., D'Esposito, M., et al. (2011). Conduction aphasia, sensory-motor integration, and phonological short-term memory-an aggregate analysis of lesion and fMRI data. Brain Lang. 119, 119-128. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.12.001
Buchsbaum, B. R., Olsen, R. K., Koch, P., and Berman, K. F. (2005). Human dorsal and ventral auditory streams subserve rehearsal-based and echoic processes during verbal working memory. Neuron 48, 687-697. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.029
Burgess, N., and Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: a network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychol. Rev. 106, 551-581. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.551
Burgess, N., and Hitch, G. J. (2006). A revised model of short-term memory and long-term learning of verbal sequences. J. Mem. Lang. 55, 627-652. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.08.005
Chein, J. M., and Fiez, J. A. (2001). Dissociating verbal working memory system components using a delayed serial recall task. Cereb. Cortex 11, 1003-1014. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.11.1003
Collette, F., Majerus, S., Van der Linden, M., Dabe, P., Degueldre, C., Delfiore, G., et al. (2001). Contribution of long-term memory to verbal short-term memory tasks: a PET activation study. Memory 9, 249-259. doi: 10.1080/09658210143000056
Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information-processing system. Psychol. Bull. 104, 163-191. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.104.2.163
Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Cowan, N., Elliott, E. M., Saults, J. S., Morey, C. C., Mattox, S., Hismjatullina, A., et al. (2005). On the capacity of attention: its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cogn. Psychol. 51, 42-100. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.12.001
Cowan, N., Li, D., Moffitt, A., Becker, T. M., Martin, E. A., Saults, J. S., et al. (2011). A neural region of abstract working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2852-2863. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2011.21625
Croot, K., Patterson, K., and Hodges, J. R. (1999). Familial progressive aphasia: insights into the nature and deterioration of single word processing. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 16, 705-747. doi: 10.1080/026432999380627
Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychol. Rev. 93, 283-321. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
Dell, G. S., Reed, K. D., Adams, D. R., and Meyer, A. S. (2000). Speech errors, phonotactic constraints, and implicit learning: a study of the role of experience in language production. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 26, 1355-1367.
Desgranges, B., Matuszewski, V., Piolino, P., Chételat, G., Mézenge, F., Landeau, B., et al. (2007). Anatomical and functional alterations in Semantic Dementia: a voxel based MRI and PET study. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 1904-1913. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.08.006
Dormal, V., Dormal, G., Joassin, F., and Pesenti, M. (2012). A common right fronto-parietal network for numerosity and duration processing: an fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 1490-1501. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21300
Duhamel, J. R., and Poncet, M. (1986). Deep dysphasia in a case of phonemic deafness: role of the right hemisphere in auditory language comprehension. Neuropsychologia24, 769-779. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(86)90076-X
Duncan, J. (2010). The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behavior. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 172-179. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.004
Emrich, S. M., Riggall, A. C., LaRocque, J. J., and Postle, B. R. (2013). Distributed patters of activity in sensory cortex reflect the precision of multiple items maintained in visual short-term memory. J. Neurosci. 33, 6516-6523. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5732-12.2013.
Engle, R. W., and Kane, M. J. (2004). "Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control, " in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, ed B. H. Ross (San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press), 145-199.
Farrell, S., and Lewandowsky, S. (2002). An endogenous distributed model of ordering in serial recall. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 59-79. doi: 10.3758/BF03196257
Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Caessens, B., and Orban, G. A. (2007). Processing of abstract ordinal knowledge in the horizontal segment of the intraparietal sulcus. J. Neurosci. 27, 8592-8596. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2076-07.2007
Fiebach, C. J., Friederici, A. D., Smith, E. E., and Swinney, D. (2007). Lateral inferotemporal cortex maintains conceptual-semantic representations in verbal working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 2035-2049. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.2035
Fiebach, C. J., Rissman, J., and D'Esposito, M. (2006). Modulation of inferotemporal cortex during verbal working memory maintenance. Neuron 51, 251-261. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.007
Fougnie, D., and Marois, R. (2007). Executive working memory load induces inattentional blindness. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 14, 142-147. doi: 10.3758/BF03194041
Foygel, D., and Dell, G. S. (2000). Models of impaired lexical access in speech production. J. Mem. Lang. 43, 182-216. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2716
Freedman, M. L., and Martin, R. C. (2001). Dissociable components of short-term memory and their relation to long-term learning. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 18, 193-226.
Friederici, A. D. (2012). The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 262-268. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.001
Garrett, M. F. (1980). "Levels of processing in sentence production, " in Language Production: Speech and Talk, Vol. 1 ed B. Butterworth (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 177-220.
Gettigan, C., Warren, J. E., Eisner, F., Marshall, C. R., Shanmugalingam, P., and Scott, S. K. (2011). Neural correlates of sublexical processing in phonological working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 961-977. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21491
Good, C. D., Scahill, R. I., Fox, N. C., Ashburner, J., Friston, K., Chan, D., et al. (2002). Automatic differentiation of anatomical patterns in the human brain: validation with studies of degenerative dementias. Neuroimage 17, 26-46. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1202
Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Brambati, S. M., Ginex, V., Ogar, J., Dronkers, N. F., Marcone, A., et al. (2008). The logopenic/phonological variant of primary progressive aphasia. Neurology 17, 1227-1234. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000320506.79811.da
Gratton, C., Sreenivasan, K. K., Silver, M. A., and D'Esposito, M. (2013). Attention selectively modifies the representation of individual faces in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 33, 6979-6989. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4142-12.2013
Gupta, P. (2003). Examining the relationship between word learning, nonword repetition and immediate serial recall in adults. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 56A, 1213-1236.
Gupta, P., Lipinski, J., Abbs, B., and Lin, P. H. (2005). Serial position effects in nonword repetition. J. Mem. Lang. 53, 141-162. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.12.002
Gupta, P., and MacWhinney, B. (1997). Vocabulary acquisition and verbal short-term memory: computational and neural bases. Brain Lang. 59, 267-333. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1819
Gupta, P., and Tisdale, J. (2009). Does phonological short-term memory causally determine vocabulary learning? Toward a computational resolution of the debate. J. Mem. Lang. 61, 481-502. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.08.001
Haller, S., Radue, E. W., Erb, M., Grodd, W., and Kircher, T. (2005). Overt sentence production in event-related fMRI. Neuropsychologia 43, 807-814. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.09.007
Hamilton, A. C., and Martin, R. C. (2005). Dissociations among tasks involving inhibition: a single-case study. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5, 1-13. doi: 10.3758/CABN.5.1.1
Hamilton, A., and Martin, R. C. (2007). Proactive interference in a semantic short-term memory deficit: role of semantic and phonological relatedness. Cortex 43, 112-123. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70449-0
Hartley, T., and Houghton, G. (1996). A linguistically constrained model of short-term memory for nonwords. J. Mem. Lang. 35, 1-31. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.0001
Hautzel, H., Mottaghy, F. M., Schmidt, D., Zemb, M., Shah, N. J., Müller-Gärtner, H.-W., et al. (2002). Topographic segregation and convergence of verbal, object, shape and spatial working memory in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 323, 156-160. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00125-8
Henson, R. N. A. (1998). Short-term memory for serial order: the start-end model. Cogn. Psychol. 36, 73-137. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0685
Henson, R. N. A., Burgess, N., and Frith, C. D. (2000). Recoding, storage, rehearsal, and grouping in verbal short-term memory: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 38, 426-440. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00098-6
Herman, A. B., Houde, J. F., Vinogradov, S., and Nagarajan, S. S. (2013). Parsing the phonological loop: activation timing in the dorsal speech stream determines accuracy in speech reproduction. J. Neurosci. 33, 5439-5453. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1472-12.2013
Hickok, G. (2012). Computational neuroanatomy of speech production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 135-145.
Hickok, G., and Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 393-402. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113
Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K. E., Oxbury, S., and Funnell, E. (1992). Semantic dementia. Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain 115, 1783-1808.
Hoffman, P., Jefferies, E., Ehsan, S., Hopper, S., and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2009). Selective short-term memory deficits arise from impaired domain-general semantic control mechanisms. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 35, 137-156. doi: 10.1037/a0013985
Hoffman, P., Jefferies, E., Ehsan, S., Jones, R. W., and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2012). How does linguistic knowledge contribute to short-term memory? Contrasting effects of impaired knowledge and executive control. Aphasiology 26, 383-403. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2011.581798
Howard, D., and Franklin, S. E. (1988). Missing the Meaning. A Cognitive Neuropsychological Study of the Processing of Words by an Aphasic Patient. Cambridge, MA: MIT.
Hulme, C., Maughan, S., and Brown, G. D. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. J. Mem. Lang. 30, 685-701. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90032-F
Jacquemot, C., and Scott, S. K. (2006). What is the relationship between phonological short-term memory and speech processing? Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 480-486.
Jefferies, E., Baker, S. S., Doran, M., and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2007). Refractory effects in stroke aphasia: a consequence of poor semantic control. Neuropsychologia45, 1065-1079. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.009
Jefferies, E., Frankish, C. R., and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). Lexical and semantic binding in verbal short-term memory. J. Mem. Lang. 54, 81-98. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.001
Jefferies, E., Hoffman, P., Jones, R., and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2008). The impact of semantic impairment on verbal short-term memory in stroke aphasia and semantic dementia: a comparative study. J. Mem. Lang. 58, 66-87. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.06.004
Jefferies, E., Jones, R. W., Bateman, D., and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2005). A semantic contribution to nonword recall. Evidence for intact phonological processes in semantic dementia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 22, 183-212. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000068
Kaas, A. L., van Mier, H., Visser, M., and Goebel, R. (2013). The neural substrate for working memory of tactile surface texture. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1148-1162. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21500
Kaufmann, L., Vogel, S. E., Starke, M., Kremser, C., and Schocke, M. (2009). Numerical and non-numerical ordinality processing in children with and without developmental dyscalculia: evidence from fMRI. Cogn. Dev. 24, 486-494. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.09.001
Knott, R., Patterson, K., and Hodges, J. R. (1997). Lexical and semantic binding effects in short-term memory: evidence from semantic dementia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 14, 1165-1216. doi: 10.1080/026432997381303
Leclercq, A. L., and Majerus, S. (2010). Serial order short-term memory predicts vocabulary development: evidence from a longitudinal study. Dev. Psychol. 46, 417-427. doi: 10.1037/a0018540
Leung, H. C., Gore, J. C., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (2002). Sustained mnemonic response in the human middle frontal gyrus during on-line storage of spatial memoranda. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 659-671. doi: 10.1162/08989290260045882
Leung, H. C., Seelig, D., and Gore, J. C. (2004). The effect of memory load on cortical activity in the spatial working memory circuit. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 4, 553-563. doi: 10.3758/CABN.4.4.553
Lewis-Peacock, J. A., Drysdale, A. T., Oberauer, K., and Postle, B. R. (2012). Neural evidence for a distinction between short-term memory and the focus of attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 61-79. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00140
Lycke, C., Specht, K., Ersland, L., and Hugdahl, K. (2008). An fMRI study of phonological and spatial working memory using identical stimuli. Scand. J. Psychol. 49, 393-401.
Majerus, S. (2009). "Verbal short-term memory and temporary activation of language representations: the importance of distinguishing item and order information, " inInteractions between Short-Term and Long-Term Memory in the Verbal Domain, eds A. S. Thorn and M. Page (Hove: Psychology Press), 244-276.
Majerus, S., Attout, L., D'Argembeau, A., Degueldre, C., Fias, W., Maquet, P., et al. (2012). Attention supports verbal short-term memory via competition between dorsal and ventral attention networks. Cereb. Cortex 22, 1086-1097. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr174
Majerus, S., Norris, D., and Patterson, K. (2007a). What do patients with semantic dementia remember in verbal short-term memory? Sounds and order but not words. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 24, 131-151.
Majerus, S., Bastin, C., Poncelet, M., Van der Linden, M., Salmon, E., Collette, F., et al. (2007b). Short-term memory and the left intraparietal sulcus: focus of attention? Further evidence from a face short-term memory paradigm. Neuroimage 35, 353-367.
Majerus, S., Van der Linden, M., Poncelet, M., and Metz-Lutz, M. N. (2004a). Can phonological and semantic short-term memory be dissociated? Further evidence Landau-Kleffner Syndrome. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 21, 491-512.
Majerus, S., Van der Linden, M., Mulder, L., Meulemans, T., and Peters, F. (2004b). Verbal short-term memory reflects the sublexical organization of the phonological language network: evidence from an incidental phonotactic learning paradigm. J. Mem. Lang. 51, 297-306.
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Van der Linden, M., and Weekes, B. (2008a). Lexical learning in bilingual adults:the relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and phonological knowledge. Cognition 107, 395-419.
Majerus, S., Belayachi, S., De Smedt, B., Leclercq, A. L., Martinez, T., Schmidt, C., et al. (2008b). Neural networks for short-term memory for order differentiate high and low proficiency bilinguals. Neuroimage 42, 1698-1713.
Majerus, S., and D'Argembeau, A. (2011). Verbal short-term memory reflects the organization of long-term memory. Further evidence from short-term memory for emotional words. J. Mem. Lang. 64, 181-197. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.10.003
Majerus, S., D'Argembeau, A., Martinez, T., Belayachi, S., Van der Linden, M., Collette, F., et al. (2010). The commonality of neural networks for verbal and visual short-term memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2570-2593. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21378
Majerus, S., Heiligenstein, L., Gautherot, N., Poncelet, M., and Van der Linden, M. (2009). The impact of auditory selective attention on verbal short-term memory and vocabulary development. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 103, 66-86. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.07.004
Majerus, S., Lekeu, F., Van der Linden, M., and Salmon, E. (2001). Deep dysphasia: further evidence on the relationship between phonological short-term memory and language processing impairments. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 18, 385-410.
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Van der Linden, M., Albouy, G., Salmon, E., Sterpenich, V., et al. (2006a). The left intraparietal sulcus and verbal short-term memory: focus of attention or serial order? Neuroimage 32, 880-891.
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Greffe, C., and Van der Linden, M. (2006b). Relations between vocabulary development and verbal short-term memory: the importance of short-term memory for serial order information. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 93, 95-119.
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Elsen, B., and Van der Linden, M. (2006c). Exploring the relationship between new word learning and short-term memory for serial order recall, item recall and item recognition. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 18, 848-873.
Makuuchi, M., and Friederici, A. D. (2013). Hierarchical functional connectivity between the core language system and the working memory system. Cortex (in press). doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.01.007
Marshuetz, C., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Smith, M.-C., Jonides, J., and Noll, D. C. (2006). Working memory for order and the parietal cortex: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroscience 139, 311-316. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.071
Marshuetz, C., Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., DeGutis, J., and Chenevert, T. L. (2000). Order information in working memory: fMRI evidence for parietal and prefrontal mechanisms. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 130-144. doi: 10.1162/08989290051137459
Martin, N., Dell, G. S., Saffran, E. M., and Schwartz, M. F. (1994a). Origins of paraphasias in deep dysphasia: testing the consequences of a decay impairment to an interactive spreading activation model of lexical retrieval. Brain Lang. 47, 609-660. doi: 10.1006/brln.1994.1061
Martin, R. C., Shelton, J. R., and Yaffee, L. S. (1994b). Language processing and working memory: neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities. J. Mem. Lang. 33, 83-111. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1994.1005
Martin, N., and Saffran, E. M. (1992). A computational account of deep dysphasia: evidence from a single case study. Brain Lang. 43, 240-274. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(92)90130-7
Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., and Dell, G. S. (1996). Recovery in deep dysphasia: evidence for a relation between auditory-verbal STM capacity and lexical errors in repetition. Brain Lang. 52, 83-113. doi: 10.1006/brln.1996.0005
Martin, R. C., and He, T. (2004). Semantic short-term memory and its role in sentence processing: a replication. Brain Lang. 89, 76-82. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00300-6
Martin, R. C., Lesch, M. F., and Bartha, M. C. (1999). Independence of input and output phonology in word processing and short-term memory. J. Mem. Lang. 41, 3-29. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2637
Martin, R. C., and Romani, C. (1994). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension: a multiple-components view. Neuropsychology 8, 506-523. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.506
Martin, R. C., Wu, D., Freedman, M., Jackson, E. F., and Lesch, M. (2003). An event-related fMRI investigation of phonological versus semantic short-term memory. J. Neurolinguist. 16, 341-360. doi: 10.1016/S0911-6044(03)00025-3
Meyer, M. L., Spunt, R. P., Berkman, E. T., Taylor, S. E., and Lieberman, M. D. (2012). Evidence for social working memory from a parametric functional MRI study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 1883-1888. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121077109
Michel, F., and Andreewsky, E. (1983). Deep dysphasia: an analogue of deep dyslexia in the auditory modality. Brain Lang. 18, 212-223. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(83)90016-0
Mosse, E. K., and Jarrold, C. (2008). Hebb learning, verbal short-term memory, and the acquisition of phonological forms in children. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61, 505-514. doi: 10.1080/17470210701680779
Mummery, C. J., Patterson, K., Price, C. J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., and Hodges, J. R. (2000). A voxel-based morphometry study of semantic dementia: relationship between temporal lobe atrophy and semantic memory. Ann. Neurol. 47, 36-45.
Nairne, J. S., and Kelley, M. R. (2004). Separating item and order information through process dissociation. J. Mem. Lang. 50, 113-133. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2003.09.005
Narayanan, N., Prabhakaran, V., Bunge, S. A., Christoff, K., Fine, E. M., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2005). The role of prefrontal cortex in the maintenance of verbal working memory information: an event-related fMRI analysis. Neuropsychology 19, 223-232. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.2.223
Nee, D. E., and Jonides, J. (2008). Neural correlates of access to short-term memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 14228-14233. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0802081105
Nee, D. E., and Jonides, J. (2011). Dissociable contributions of prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus to short-term memory: evidence for a 3-state model of memory. Neuroimage 54, 1540-1548.
Nee, D. E., and Jonides, J. (2013). Neural evidence for a 3-state model of visual short-term memory. Neuroimage 74, 1-11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.019
Nystrom, L. E., Braver, T. S., Sabb, F. W., Delgado, M. R., Noll, D. C., and Cohen, J. D. (2000). Working memory for letters, shapes, and locations: fMRI evidence against stimulus-based regional organization in human prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 11, 424-446. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0572
Oberauer, K. (2002). Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 28, 411-421. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.411
Ötzekin, I., Davachi, L., and McElree, B. (2010). Are representations in working memory distinct from representations in long-term memory?: neural evidence in support of a single store. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1123-1133. doi: 10.1177/0956797610376651
Pa, J., Wilson, S. M., Pickell, H., Bellugi, U., and Hickok, G. (2008). Neural organization of linguistic short-term memory is sensory modality-dependent: evidence from signed and spoken language. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 2198-2210. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20154
Page, M. P. A., Cumming, N., Madge, A., and Norris, D. (2007). Speech errors and the phonological similarity effect in short-term memory: evidence suggesting a common locus. J. Mem. Lang. 56, 49-64. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.09.002
Page, M. P. A., and Norris, D. (1998). The Primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. Psychol. Rev. 105, 761-781. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.105.4.761-781
Papagno, C., Vernice, M., and Cecchetto, C. (2013). Phonology without semantics? Good enough for verbal short-term memory. Evidence from a patient with semantic dementia. Cortex 49, 626-636. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.015
Papoutsi, M., de Zwart, J. A., Jansma, J. M., Pickering, M. J., Bednar, J. A., and Horwitz, B. (2009). From phonemes to articulatory codes: an fMRI study of the role of Broca's area in speech production. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2156-2165. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn239
Patterson, K. E., Graham, N., and Hodges, J. R. (1994). The impact of semantic memory loss on phonological representations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 57-69. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1994.6.1.57
Paulesu, E., Frith, C. D., and Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1993). The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature 362, 342-345. doi: 10.1038/362342a0
Pinel, P. H., Dehaene, S., Rivière, D., and LeBihan, D. (2001). Modulation of parietal activation by semantic distance in a number comparison task. Neuroimage 14, 1013-1026. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0913
Poirier, M., and Saint-Aubin, J. (1996). Immediate serial recall, word frequency, item identity and item position. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 50, 408-412. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.50.4.408
Postle, B. R. (2005). Delay-period activity in the prefrontal cortex: one function is sensory gating. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 1679-1690. doi: 10.1162/089892905774589208
Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience 139, 23-28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005
Rämä, P., Sala, J. B., Gillen, J. S., Pekar, J. J., and Courtney, S. M. (2001). Dissociation of the neural systems for working memory maintenance of verbal and nonspatial visual information. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 1, 161-171. doi: 10.3758/CABN.1.2.161
Ranganath, C., Cohen, M. X., Dam, C., and D'Esposito, M. (2004). Inferior temporal, prefrontal, and hippocampal contributions to visual working memory maintenance and associative memory retrieval. J. Neurosci. 24, 3917-3925. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5053-03.2004
Rauschecker, J. P., and Scott, S. K. (2009). Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 718-724. doi: 10.1038/nn.2331
Ravizza, S. M., Delgado, M. R., Chein, J. M., Becker, J. T., and Fiez, J. A. (2004). Functional dissociations within the inferior parietal cortex in verbal working memory. Neuroimage 22, 562-573. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.039
Rypma, B., and D'Esposito, M. (1999). The roles of prefrontal brain regions in components of working memory: effects of memory load and individual differences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 6558-6563. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.11.6558
Rypma, B., Berger, J. S., and D'Esposito, M. (2002). The influence of working memory demand and subject performance on prefrontal cortical activity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 721-731. doi: 10.1162/08989290260138627
Salmon, E., Van der Linden, M., Collette, F., Delfiore, G., Maquet, P., Degueldre, C., et al. (1996). Regional brain activity during working memory tasks. Brain 119, 1617-1625. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.5.1617
Schnur, T. T., Schwartz, M. F., Kimberg, D. Y., Hirshorn, E., Coslett, H. B., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009). Localizing interference during naming: convergent neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence for the function of Broca's area. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 322-327. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805874106
Scott, S. K., Catrin Blank, C., Rosen, S., and Wise, R. J. S. (2000). Identification of a pathway for intelligible speech in the left temporal lobe. Brain 123, 2400-2406. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.12.2400
Segaert, K., Kempen, G., Petersson, K. M., and Hagoort, P. (2013). Syntactic priming and the lexical boost effect during sentence production and sentence comprehension: an fMRI study. Brain Lang. 124, 174-183. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.12.003
Strand, F., Forssberg, H., Klingberg, T., and Norrelgen, F. (2008). Phonological working memory with auditory presentation of pseudo-words: an event related fMRI Study. Brain Res. 1212, 48-54. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.02.097
Takayama, Y., Kinomoto, K., and Nakamura, K. (2004). Selective impairment of the auditory-verbal short-term memory due to a lesion of the superior temporal gyrus. Eur. Neurol. 51, 115-117. doi: 10.1159/000076792
Thompson-Schill, S. L., Jonides, J., Marshuetz, C., Smith, E. E., D'Esposito, M., Kan, I. P., et al. (2002). Effects of frontal lobe damage on interference effects in working memory. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2, 109-120. doi: 10.3758/CABN.2.2.109
Todd, J. J., Fougnie, D., and Marois, R. (2005). Visual short-term memory load suppresses temporo-parietal junction activity and induces inattentional blindness. Psychol. Sci. 16, 965-972. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01645.x
Todd, J. J., and Marois, R. (2004). Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature 428, 751-754. doi: 10.1038/nature02466
Tree, J. J., Perfect, T. J., Hirsh, K. W., and Copstick, S. (2001). Deep dysphasic performance in non-fluent progressive aphasia: a case study. Neurocase 7, 473-488. doi: 10.1093/neucas/7.6.473
Treiman, R., and Danis, C. (1988). Short-term memory errors for spoken syllables are affected by the linguistic structure of the syllables. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 14, 145-152. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.14.1.145
Tremblay, P., and Small, S. L. (2011). Motor response selection in overt sentence production: a functional MRI study. Front. Psychol. 2:253. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00253
Trojano, L., Stanzione, M., and Grossi, D. (1992). Short-term memory and verbal learning with auditory phonological coding defect: a neuropsychological case study. Brain Cogn. 18, 12-33. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(92)90108-X
Vallar, G., Basso, A., and Bottini, G. (1990). "Phonological processing and sentence comprehension: a neuropsychological case study, " in Neuropsychological Impairments of Short-Term Memory, eds G. Vallar and T. Shallice (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 448-476. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511665547.022
Vallar, G., and Papagno, C. (2002). "Neuropsychological impairments of verbal short-term memory, " in The Handbook of Memory Disorders, eds A. D. Baddeley, M. D. Kopelman, and B. A. Wilson (Chichester: Wiley), 249-270.
Van Dijck, J.-P. H., Abrahamse, E., Majerus, S., and Fias, W. (2013). Spatial attention interacts with serial order retrieval in verbal working memory. Psychol. Sci. (in press).
Vitevitch, M. S., and Luce, P. A. (1999). Probabilistic phonotactics and neighborhood activation in spoken word recognition. J. Mem. Lang. 40, 374-408. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2618
Warrington, E. K., Logue, V., and Pratt, R. T. C. (1971). The anatomical localisation of selective impairment of auditory verbal short-term memory. Neuropsychologia 9, 377-387. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90002-9
Wilshire, C., and Fisher, C. (2004). "Phonological" dysphasia: a cross-modal phonological impairment affecting repetition, production, and comprehension. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 21, 187-210. doi: 10.1080/02643290342000555
Similar publications
Sorry the service is unavailable at the moment. Please try again later.
This website uses cookies to improve user experience. Read more
Save & Close
Accept all
Decline all
Show detailsHide details
Cookie declaration
About cookies
Strictly necessary
Performance
Strictly necessary cookies allow core website functionality such as user login and account management. The website cannot be used properly without strictly necessary cookies.
This cookie is used by Cookie-Script.com service to remember visitor cookie consent preferences. It is necessary for Cookie-Script.com cookie banner to work properly.
Performance cookies are used to see how visitors use the website, eg. analytics cookies. Those cookies cannot be used to directly identify a certain visitor.
Used to store the attribution information, the referrer initially used to visit the website
Cookies are small text files that are placed on your computer by websites that you visit. Websites use cookies to help users navigate efficiently and perform certain functions. Cookies that are required for the website to operate properly are allowed to be set without your permission. All other cookies need to be approved before they can be set in the browser.
You can change your consent to cookie usage at any time on our Privacy Policy page.